RESEARCH ARTICLE
A d factor? Understanding trait distractibility
and its relationships with ADHD
symptomatology and hyperfocus
Han Zhang
ID
1
*, Akira Miyake
2
, Jahla Osborne
ID
1
, Priti Shah
1
, John Jonides
1
1 Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, United States of America,
2 Department of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO, United States
of America
Abstract
People differ substantially in their vulnerability to distraction. Yet, many types of distractions
exist, from external stimulation to internal thoughts. How should we characterize individual
differences in their distractibility? Two samples of adult participants (total N = 1220) com-
pleted a large battery of questionnaires assessing different facets of real-world distractibility.
Latent modeling revealed that these measures could be explained by three correlated-yet-
distinct factors: external distraction, unwanted intrusive thoughts, and mind-wandering.
Importantly, about 80% of the total variance in these three factors could be explained by a
single higher-order factor (d) that could be construed in terms of a person’s general distracti-
bility, and this general distractibility model was replicated across the two samples. We then
applied the general distractibility model to understand the nature of ADHD symptomatology
and hyperfocus (an intense state of long-lasting and highly focused attention). d was sub-
stantially associated with self-reported ADHD symptoms. Interestingly, d was also positively
associated with hyperfocus, suggesting that hyperfocus may, to some degree, reflect atten-
tion problems. These results also show marked consistencies across the two samples.
Overall, the study provides an important step toward a comprehensive understanding of
individual differences in distractibility and related constructs.
Introduction
Distraction is prevalent in our daily lives. As you read this article, you might be distracted by
the voices of people around you, by worries about an impending decision on a grant applica-
tion, or by fantasies about your next holiday travel plan.
Distraction has been a central topic in the study of cognition for well over 100 years [1]. In
this literature, one central theme has been that people differ substantially in how easily they get
distracted. Numerous studies have shown that people who report being distracted more easily
are at a higher risk of poor performance in school and work settings [2, 3] and even sometimes
prone to serious accidents [4]. The issue of interindividual variability also cuts across multiple
forms of psychopathology, most notably as a symptom of attention-deficit/hyperactivity
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 1 / 31
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Zhang H, Miyake A, Osborne J, Shah P,
Jonides J (2023) A d factor? Understanding trait
distractibility and its relationships with ADHD
symptomatology and hyperfocus. PLoS ONE
18(10): e0292215. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.
pone.0292215
Editor: Ioanna Markostamou, University of
Hertfordshire, UNITED KINGDOM
Received: July 16, 2023
Accepted: September 11, 2023
Published: October 25, 2023
Copyright: © 2023 Zhang et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: All data, code, and
study materials are available at https://osf.io/8j6p4/
.
Funding: This work was supported by the National
Science Foundation [grant number: 1658268]
awarded to the University of Michigan with JJ as
Principal Investigator and the National Institute of
Mental Health (Unique Federal Award Identification
Number (FAIN): R21MH129909) awarded to the
University of Michigan with JJ as Principal
Investigator. The funders had no role in study
disorder (ADHD) but also featured in other psychiatric disorders including anxiety, depres-
sion, and schizophrenia [59]. Thus, understanding individual differences in distractibility is
important not only for predicting for whom attention is likely to fail but also for understand-
ing the cognitive underpinnings of psychopathology.
How can we best characterize individual differences in distractibility? Surprisingly, there is
no clear, comprehensive answer to this question yet, because there are multiple different facets
to distractibility. Distractibility can be conceptualized to mean susceptibility to irrelevant stim-
ulation [5], the tendency to have thought intrusions [10], the tendency to experience repetitive
negative thinking [11], or the tendency to engage in mind-wandering [12]. Oftentimes, how-
ever, these different conceptualizations of distractibility have been studied in isolation; in fact,
most existing studies on this topic consider only a single aspect of distractibility (e.g., external
distraction) and often assess it with a single measure. Moreover, studies on distractibility are
often conducted with underpowered samples (most often with just convenience samples of
college students) and without replication of the results. For these reasons, it is yet unclear
whether and to what extent these constructs reflect different facets of distraction.
The current study used an individual-differences approach to establish a structure of dis-
tractibility. We assessed different facets of distractibility in two large samples (one drawn from
college students and the other drawn from the broader community) by administering a large
battery of self-report measures, each facet represented by multiple questionnaires. Using con-
firmatory factor analyses, we specified how these constructs are empirically related to one
another at the latent level. Then, we applied our resulting model to understand individual dif-
ferences in ADHD symptomatology as well as hyperfocus, a prolonged state of concentration
often observed among individuals with high levels of ADHD symptomatology [13]. As such,
the current study sheds new light not only on the factor structure of individual differences in
different types of distractibility but also on the nature of distractibility associated with ADHD.
Different forms of distraction
The term “distraction” is commonly used to refer to irrelevant percepts in the external envi-
ronment, such as irrelevant visual stimuli (e.g., a visually salient object unrelated to a current
visual search) and irrelevant auditory stimuli (e.g., irrelevant speech). The study of external
distraction has a long and distinguished history in the field of psychology featuring experimen-
tal manipulations of external stimulation [1416]. Furthermore, the notion of distractibility as
an important individual-differences factor has long been proposed, but “distractibility” in
these early studies almost exclusively refers to distraction by external stimulation [5, 1720].
However, external stimulation is likely not the only source of distraction; there is increasing
evidence showing that performance can be hampered by information that is hypothesized to
have an internal origin [2123]. Three notable constructs that are conceptually distinguishable
from external distraction are (a) thought intrusions, (b) repetitive negative thinking, and (c)
mind-wandering.
Thought intrusion is a common experience. Studies have found that over 80% of healthy
individuals experience some form of intrusive thoughts [24]. The contents of these thoughts
are often negative (e.g., an unpleasant memory) and sometimes even abhorrent and disgusting
(e.g., violence against someone). Though typically studied from a psychopathology perspective,
thought intrusions constitute a form of cognitive distraction because they interrupt the flow of
thought and capture mental capacity. As a result, one of the hallmarks of thought intrusions is
that they interfere with ongoing task performance [25].
Repetitive negative thinking is defined as “repetitive thinking about one or more negative
topics that is experienced as difficult to control” [26, p.193]. Two common forms of repetitive
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 2 / 31
design, data collection and analysis, decision to
publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
negative thinking are rumination, defined as past-oriented persistent dwelling on causes and
consequences of one’s distress [27], and worry, defined as future-oriented repetitive thinking
about potential threats, uncertainties, and risks [28, 29]. Both rumination and worry are asso-
ciated with difficulty concentrating and low task performance [21, 23, 30]. Although repetitive
negative thinking and thought intrusions share many similarities (e.g., unpleasant, distracting,
and uncontrollable), there are some fine-grained differences between them. Clark and Rhyno
[24] suggest that repetitive negative thinking may represent a more persistent form of cogni-
tive interference, whereas thought intrusions may be more sudden, undirected, and somewhat
unexpected (akin to a “mental flash”).
The term mind-wandering was used by Smallwood and Schooler [12] to describe a collec-
tion of terms that all entail a spontaneous shift of attention away from a task toward “unrelated
inner thoughts, fantasies, feelings, and other musings” (p. 946). The term has since been
employed to characterize a diverse array of mental phenomena, including intentional or delib-
erate forms of mind-wandering (as opposed to unintentional or spontaneous), and unguided
thoughts that may arise even in the absence of any task [31]. Considering the expansive usage
of the term, not all mental phenomena grouped under it can be deemed as distractions. In the
study we report here, we narrows its focus on the facet of mind-wandering that is spontaneous
and unrelated to the task at hand. Under this definition, mind-wandering constitutes a form of
distraction in many cognitive tasks [32].
In conception, for several reasons, mind-wandering is not considered as simply repetitive
negative thinking or thought intrusion. First, mind-wandering is not necessarily negative or
unpleasant. For example, during a lecture, students may find themselves thinking about unre-
lated things such as their plans for the weekend. While these thoughts disrupt attention to the
lecture, they may be quite pleasant, interesting, and constructive for other purposes [33]. Sec-
ond, mind-wandering does not necessarily involve recurrent, repetitive, or cyclical mental
content [34]. A person may meander from topic to topic during an episode of mind-wander-
ing; different mind-wandering episodes may also involve wholly different topics. Third, indi-
viduals are often unaware of their mind-wandering state [35] whereas repetitive negative
thinking and thought intrusion are often associated with conscious appraisals and attempts to
resist [36].
The relationships among facets of distractibility
We have reviewed several constructs that capture different aspects of how easily a person can
be distracted, but how exactly are they related to each other? That is, if a person is prone to one
type of distraction, are they also prone to other types of distractions? To our knowledge, no
prior work has examined the relationships among all of these constructs simultaneously within
a single study. Without an empirical assessment of their relationships, we run the risk of jingle-
jangle fallacies [37, 38]. In the jingle fallacy, the same term is used to refer to distinct con-
structs, whereas in the jangle fallacy, different terms are used to refer to the same construct.
At one extreme, for example, external distraction, thought intrusions, repetitive negative
thinking, and mind-wandering may be completely separable and not share anything in com-
mon, thus making the practice of calling them all instances of distraction highly misleading
(representing a case of the jingle fallacy). At the other extreme, they may be completely redun-
dant constructs (thus, representing a case of the jangle fallacy) so there is no need to differenti-
ate among them.
Both of these accounts are unlikely, however, in light of previous studies examining the
relationship between external distraction and mind-wandering, which have shown that they
are correlated yet distinct constructs [39, 40]. For example, Unsworth and McMillan [40]
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 3 / 31
obtained self-reports of external distraction and mind-wandering by presenting thought
probes as participants completed laboratory tasks. Using confirmatory factor analysis, they
found that a two-factor model consisting of an external distraction factor and a mind-
wandering factor fit statistically better than combining them into one factor (i.e., constraining
their correlation to 1), although there was a sizable correlation between the two factors
(r = .44). While these studies show that external distraction and mind-wandering demonstrate
some overlap, they did not distinguish mind-wandering from similar constructs like thought
intrusions and repetitive negative thinking. Instead, “mind-wandering” was used generally to
represent distraction by internal stimuli [40].
Indeed, the practice of partitioning distraction into an external-internal dichotomy is preva-
lent in the literature [4144]. By this account, thought intrusions, repetitive negative thinking,
and mind-wandering should be highly correlated (if not redundant) and load onto the same fac-
tor because, in conception, they all reflect distraction by internal thoughts. However, studies
have shown that mind-wandering can be experienced as quite pleasant even though it may
reduce task performance [33]. Furthermore, several studies have shown that self-reported
mind-wandering tendency is only moderately correlated with self-reported thought intrusions
(rs = .24 to.36 [45, 46]). These results suggest that what is perceived as mind-wandering might
not be perceived as repetitive negative thinking (or thought intrusion). If so, then a simple exter-
nal-internal dichotomy may not adequately capture the relationships among these constructs.
A general distractibility factor?
The studies reviewed above suggest that external distraction, thought intrusions, repetitive
negative thinking, and mind-wandering may have some underlying commonality but also
demonstrate some separability, a pattern similar to that observed in individual differences
studies of executive functions [47, 48]. This pattern of “unity and diversity” provides a useful
way to think about the factor structure of distractibility. It may be plausible to assume that the
correlations among various forms of distraction reflect an overarching trait that governs a per-
son’s general tendency to be distracted by irrelevant information. Additionally, there might be
additional factors that determine a person’s vulnerability to a specific form of distraction. This
unity/diversity specification of distractibility seems to map well onto existing theories [9, 49
53]. For example, it has been argued that a person’s domain-general working memory capacity
plays an overarching role in maintaining the task goal and avoiding distractions [51]. Other
specific factors, such as current life concerns, play an additional role in determining the occur-
rence of mind-wandering.
Several recent studies have attempted to establish a general trait of distractibility, most nota-
bly those conducted by Forster and Lavie [54, 55]. These authors created a laboratory task that
was designed to parallel external distractions in daily life. In this task, external distraction was
introduced by the presence of irrelevant cartoon figures in a visual search task. The dependent
measure was a distractor-interference score based on the RT difference between distractor-
absent and distractor-present conditions. Forster and Lavie [54, 55] found that the distractor
interference score was correlated with a self-reported measure of mind-wandering (r = .26
(Exp. 1) and r = .38 (Exp. 3)) and with a self-reported measure of childhood ADHD symptoms
(r = .32 (Exp. 1) and r = .32 (Exp. 2)). Based on these results, Forster and Lavie [55] suggested
that a general factor exists that “confers vulnerability to irrelevant distraction across the gen-
eral population” (p. 209). However, a subsequent study failed to replicate those findings [56].
Of particular concern is the low internal consistency of the cartoon-distraction task (.08—.26
as reported in [56]), which suggests that the task might not reliably capture individual differ-
ences in external distractibility.
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 4 / 31
In another study, Hobbiss et al. [57] assessed the degree to which people experience external
distractions and mind-wandering while engaging in a wide range of daily life activities. Hob-
biss et al. [57] found that self-reports of external distraction were correlated with self-reports
of mind-wandering and that these measures loaded on a single factor. However, because there
were seven external distraction items and a single mind-wandering item, the resulting single
factor still largely captured external distraction rather than what is common to external dis-
traction and mind-wandering.
Overall, although these recent studies provided some initial evidence for a potential higher-
order distractibility factor, the evidence is still limited. Moreover, in any of the studies just
reviewed here, two other forms of distraction—thought intrusions and repetitive negative
thinking—were not considered. Thus, it is necessary to further investigate the notion of a gen-
eral distractibility factor in a more comprehensive manner.
Linking distractibility to ADHD symptoms and hyperfocus
By extracting a general distractibility factor, one can directly examine how the unity and diver-
sity components of distractibility are related to other individual-differences variables. This is
important because, if a variable of interest is related to multiple correlated factors, this relation-
ship could be driven by just the general component or both the general and the specific com-
ponents. We will take advantage of this feature to understand individual differences in
behavioral symptoms and functional impairments associated with ADHD as well as
hyperfocus.
ADHD symptoms and associated functional impairments. Heightened distractibility
has often been associated with ADHD, defined as an ongoing pattern of inattention and/or
hyperactivity-impulsivity that interferes with functioning or development [58]. Though only
about 4% of adults have a formal diagnosis of ADHD, ADHD symptoms are prevalent in the
general population, and a formal diagnosis represents the extreme end of a continuous distri-
bution [59, 60]. ADHD symptoms are associated with functional impairments in various life
domains, including school, work, and interpersonal relationships [6163]. One of the diagnos-
tic criteria of ADHD is “being often easily distracted by extraneous stimuli (for older adoles-
cents and adults, may include unrelated thoughts)” [58].
What is the nature of the relationship between distractibility and ADHD? The separate
associations between ADHD symptomatology and different forms of distractions may, in
fact, be driven by a single factor that represents an individual’s overall distractibility level.
However, it has been difficult to assess the role of a general factor in explaining ADHD
symptoms because most studies have examined a single form of distraction in relation to
ADHD.
Much of the research on distractibility and ADHD has focused on external distraction [15,
6466], most likely because “distraction” has traditionally been studied in terms of distraction
by extraneous stimuli. However, separate studies have reported that ADHD symptomatology
is also associated with vulnerability to unwanted intrusive thoughts [67] and spontaneous
mind-wandering [41, 43, 68]. Because different forms of distraction were not considered
simultaneously in these studies, it remains difficult to specify the nature of these observed
correlations.
To what extent are these correlations driven by a common factor that is elevated among
people who have high levels of ADHD symptomatology? Is there anything special between
ADHD and certain forms of distraction beyond general distractibility? One way to address
these questions is to assess whether the relationship between a particular form of distraction
and ADHD still holds after partialing out a possible general factor. If there is a special
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 5 / 31
relationship between a particular form of distraction and ADHD symptoms, one should expect
a meaningful relationship even after controlling for the general factor.
Hyperfocus. The term hyperfocus is used to describe episodes of long-lasting and highly
focused attention that people with high levels of ADHD symptomatology often report [69, 70].
For example, although these individuals often have difficulties paying attention in lectures or
getting things in order, they may find themselves spending hours writing computer programs,
watching TV, and getting engrossed in creative thoughts, to the extent that they completely
lose track of time or forget their personal needs. The term has also attracted huge interest in
the general public with frequent discussions in the media [7173]. Despite this interest, the
nature of hyperfocus remains poorly understood.
At face value, distractibility and hyperfocus seem to be opposite constructs. The former
entails a state of distracted attention, whereas the latter entails a state of deeply focused atten-
tion. Thus, it seems plausible that distractibility and hyperfocus are not correlated at all or
even negatively correlated. However, there are reasons to believe that distractibility is positively
correlated with hyperfocus. For example, people might be distracted by the same sorts of sub-
jects on which they might tend to hyperfocus. Indeed, hyperfocus is sometimes (but not
always) experienced as negative, such as wasting time on unimportant activities, having diffi-
culty switching to more urgent tasks, and getting “stuck” on small details [13]. If so, then those
who have a higher tendency to engage in hyperfocus might be more easily getting stuck on
subjects that distract them.
An even more intriguing possibility is that hyperfocus is differentially associated with dif-
ferent facets of distractibility. Many instances of hyperfocus experiences include common
qualities such as distorted time perception, failure to attend to the world, ignoring personal
needs, and feelings of total engrossment [13]. These descriptions seem to be antagonistic to
external distraction, which presumably involves engaging in some sensory processing. Simi-
larly, it is plausible to think that one is less capable of remaining in a hyperfocus state if
unwanted thoughts frequently intrude into their current train of thought. In contrast, various
cases of hyperfocus seem to share certain similarities with those of mind-wandering. Mind-
wandering has been viewed as a state of “decoupled attention”, which entails an inward shift of
attention to one’s thoughts and feelings and, as a result, an attenuation of sensory processing
[12, 35].
As such, hyperfocus seems to share some similarities with mind-wandering that are not
shared with other forms of distraction. Then, general distractibility alone may not sufficiently
explain the nature of hyperfocus; it may have more nuanced relationships with specific compo-
nents of distractibility beyond what could be accounted for by a possible general factor.
The current study
There were two primary goals for the current study. The first goal was to understand the rela-
tionships among several popular constructs that tap into distractibility. These constructs
included external distraction, thought intrusions, repetitive negative thinking, and mind-
wandering. To our knowledge, no prior work has examined the relationships among all of
these constructs simultaneously within a single study. Using latent variable analyses, we
assessed the extent to which these constructs, each measured by multiple instruments, are
redundant or separable at the latent level. We also examined the plausibility of extracting a
general factor to capture the commonality underlying these constructs.
The second goal of the study was to apply the model of distractibility derived from the
study to understand the nature of ADHD symptomatology and hyperfocus. We hypothesized
that the putative general factor should account for substantial variance in self-reported
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 6 / 31
symptoms of ADHD and associated functional impairments. But it remains to be seen whether
it has any meaningful relationship with hyperfocus. In addition to examining the role of the
general factor, we were also interested in assessing the role of construct-specific variances in
explaining ADHD symptoms, functional impairments, and hyperfocus. To do so, we exam-
ined how much additional variance the specific distractibility factors could explain over and
above the general factor.
We addressed these two goals from a trait perspective by using questionnaires that measure
individuals’ tendencies to experience various forms of distraction in daily life. Recent studies
suggest that, compared to task-based (behavioral) measures, self-report questionnaire-based
measures tend to converge better and be more associated with real-life behaviors [7476]. This
does not necessarily indicate that questionnaires are superior to tasks in terms of measuring
distractibility, but establishing the relationships among a range of self-reported real-world dis-
tractions appears to be a reasonable starting point toward a more comprehensive understand-
ing of distractibility. Although specifying the extent of convergence between task-derived and
questionnaire-derived factors of distractibility is an important question, it is beyond the scope
of the present study.
Given the exploratory nature of our research goals, we conducted an internal replication.
Specifically, we collected data from two large samples (Ns = 651 and 569) and used the first
sample for exploratory analyses and the second sample for replication. The two samples dif-
fered in several dimensions, including age, gender distribution, racial composition, and occu-
pation. As such, the results of the second sample help evaluate the replicability of our findings.
Methods
Below we report how we determined our sample size and, all data exclusions and measures in
the study [77]. All data, code, and study materials are available at https://osf.io/8j6p4/.
The procedure was approved by University of Michigan Health Sciences and Behavioral
Sciences Institutional Review Board (IRB-HSBS), study ID: HUM00066883. Because both
samples were collected online, written consent could not be obtained from the participants.
Instead, we presented a digital version of the consent form at the beginning of the study and
offered participants to select whether they agree to participate or not. Only those who provided
their agreement were then given access to the full study.
Participants
For both samples, we recruited as many participants as possible given the resources available.
It is worth noting that the study was conducted from October 2020 to February 2021, a period
during which COVID-19 cases were rapidly increasing in the US and in-person testing was
not feasible. Therefore, both samples were recruited using online platforms.
Sample 1. We recruited participants from Prolifc.co that met the following criteria: (a)
between 18 and 35 years of age; (b) residing in the U.S. or Canada; (c) native speakers of
English; (d) with 50–3000 previous submissions and 95%-100% approval rate on the Prolific.
co platform. Participants were compensated with $3.67 for participation. Data were discarded
if participants failed 2 or more out of the 4 attention-check questions interspersed throughout
the questionnaires (n = 5). We further removed data from 1 participant who had a duplicate IP
address. The final sample size was 651, with a mean age of 26.9 (SD = 5.0), 45.3% female,
67.7% white, and 33.9% current students.
Sample 2. We recruited 615 students from the introductory psychology subject pool at
the University of Michigan. Participants completed the study remotely and received partial
course credit for participation. Data were discarded if participants failed 2 or more out of the 4
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 7 / 31
attention check questions (n = 46). Thus, the final sample size was 569, with a mean age of 18.8
(SD = 1.2), 66.3% female, and 56.8% white.
Measures
A battery of commonly used self-report instruments was administered to assess a person’s
vulnerability to external distraction, thought intrusions, repetitive negative thinking, and
mind-wandering. Example items of each measure can be found in Table 1.
Imaginal Processes Inventory—Distractibility (IPI-D). The Imaginal Processes Inven-
tory—Distractibility subscale is a 5-item scale originally designed by Singer and Antrobus [78]
and later refined by Giambra [79] to measure task distractibility with competing stimulation.
Items were rated on a 0 (definitely not true for me) to 4 (very true for me) scale. The final
score was the average score of the 5 items, with a higher score indicating greater vulnerability
to external distraction.
Attentional Style Questionnaire—External (ASQ-E). The Attentional Style Question-
naire measures the ability to maintain attention on task-related stimuli and not to be distracted
by interfering stimuli [80]. The external distraction subscale has 5 items measuring susceptibil-
ity to external interfering stimuli. Items were rated on a 1 (in total disagreement) to 6 (in total
agreement) scale. We administered the full scale but used only the external subscale in data
analysis because the Internal subscale was not designed to measure internal distractions
Table 1. Scale names and example items.
Scale Example Item
External Distraction
Imaginal Processes Inventory—
Distractibility
I find it difficult to concentrate when the TV or radio is on.
Attentional Style Questionnaire—
External
I have trouble concentrating when there is movement in the room I am in.
Attentional Control—Distraction I have difficulty concentrating when there is music in the room around me.
Thought Intrusions
White Bear Suppression
Inventory
There are thoughts that keep jumping into my head.
Thought Control Ability
Questionnaire
It is very easy for me to stop having certain thoughts.
Thought Suppression Inventory I have thoughts which I would rather not have.
Repetitive Negative Thinking
Perseverative Thinking
Questionnaire
The same thoughts keep going through my mind again and again.
Penn State Worry Questionnaire I am always worrying about something.
Ruminative Response Scale—
Brooding
[How often do you] think “What am I doing to deserve this?”
Mind-Wandering
Imaginal Processes Inventory—
Mind-wandering
During a lecture or speech, my mind often wanders.
Mind-wandering—Spontaneous I find my thoughts wandering spontaneously.
Daydreaming Frequency Scale I lose myself in active daydreaming [frequency option].
ADHD-related Scales
Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale How often do you have problems remembering appointments or obligations?
Functional Impairments [ADHD symptoms affect my ability to function] In my work or occupation.
Adult Dispositional Hyperfocus
Questionnaire
Generally, when I am busy doing something I enjoy or something that I am
very focused on, I tend to completely lose track of time.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215.t001
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 8 / 31
specifically (e.g., “It is hard for me to stay on one activity for a whole hour.”). The final score
was the average score of the 5 items in the external subscale. A higher score indicated greater
vulnerability to external distraction.
Attentional Control—Distraction (AC-D). The Attentional Control—Distraction scale
is a 4-item scale measuring susceptibility to external distraction [81]. Items were rated on a 1
(almost never) to 5 (always) scale. The final score was the average of the 4 items, with a higher
score indicating greater vulnerability to external distraction.
White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI). The White Bear Suppression Inventory is a
15-item scale measuring the control of thoughts [82]. Items were rated on a 1 (strongly dis-
agree) to 5 (strongly agree) scale. The final score was the average score of all items, with a
higher score indicating greater vulnerability to thought intrusions.
Thought Control Ability Questionnaire (TCAQ). The Thought Control Ability Ques-
tionnaire is a 20-item scale measuring one’s ability to control unwanted thoughts [83]. Items
were rated on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) scale. To make the directionality of
this scale consistent with that of the other scales, we reversed the original scoring scheme so
that a higher score indicated worse thought control ability. The final score was the average
score of all items.
Thought Suppression Inventory (TSI). The Thought Suppression Inventory is an
18-item scale measuring thought suppression [84]. Items were rated on a 1 (strongly disagree)
to 5 (strongly agree) scale. Previous research has identified three subscales: intrusion, suppres-
sion attempt, and effective suppression [85]. We administered the full scale but excluded items
in the suppression attempt subscale from data analysis because a suppression attempt can be
either successful or unsuccessful, making it an ambiguous measure of thought suppression.
The final score was the average score of all the intrusion items and the reverse-coded effective
suppression items. A higher score indicated greater thought intrusion.
Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ). The Perseverative Thinking Question-
naire is a 15-item clinical measure of the extent to which individuals experience repetitive,
uncontrollable, and attention-consuming thoughts [86]. Items were rated on a 0 (never) to 4
(almost always) scale. The final score was the average score of the 15 items. A higher score
indicated a greater tendency to engage in repetitive negative thinking.
Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ). The Penn State Worry Questionnaire is a
16-item clinical measure of excessive and uncontrollable worry [87]. Items were rated on a 1
(not at all typical of me) to 5 (very typical of me) scale. The final score was the average of the
16 items, with a higher score indicating a greater tendency to worry.
Ruminative Response Scale—Brooding (RRS-B). The Ruminative Response Scale
(short form) is a 10-item clinical measure of the reflection and brooding aspects of rumina-
tion [88]. The reflection subscale and brooding subscales each have 5 items. Participants
were asked to read each item and indicate the extent to which they think or do each one
when they feel down, sad, or depressed. Items were rated on a 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost
always) scale. Previous research has shown that the brooding subscale is more representative
of maladaptive thinking whereas the reflection subscale captures a less maladaptive form of
rumination [88]. Thus, the brooding subscale was used as the manifest variable of repetitive
negative thinking, rather than the full scale. The final score was the average of the 5 items in
the brooding subscale, with a higher score indicating a greater tendency to engage in
brooding.
Imaginal Processes Inventory—Mind-wandering (IPI-MW). The Imaginal Processes
Inventory—Mind-wandering subscale is a 6-item scale proposed by [79] using items originally
from the Imaginal Processes Inventory [78]. The IPI-MW measures task-unrelated thoughts
during tasks. Items were rated from 0 (definitely not true for me) to 4 (very true for me). The
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 9 / 31
final score was the average of the 6 items, with a higher score indicating a greater tendency to
engage in mind-wandering.
Mind-wandering—Spontaneous (MW-S). The Mind-wandering—Spontaneous scale is a
4-item scale measuring spontaneous mind-wandering [81]. Items were rated on a 1 (rarely) to
7 (a lot) scale except for the 3rd item (“It feels like I don’t have control over when my mind
wanders.”), which was rated on a 1 (almost never) to 7 (almost always) scale. The final score
was the average of the 4 items, with a higher score indicating a greater tendency to engage in
spontaneous mind-wandering.
Daydreaming Frequency Scale (DDFS). The Daydreaming Frequency Scale is a 13-item
scale originally from the Imaginal Processes Inventory [78]. The original scale has 12 items,
but according to the factor analysis by [79], an additional item (“I tend to get pretty wrapped up
in my daydreaming.”) should belong to this scale. Items were rated on a 1-to-5 scale. The final
score was the average of all items, with a higher score indicating a greater tendency to engage
in daydreaming.
Adult ADHD Self-report Scale. The Adult ADHD Self-report Scale is a popular 18-item
clinical measure for screening adults with ADHD [89]. The 18 items correspond to the eighteen
criteria used to diagnose ADHD. Each question was rated on a 5-point scale from 0 (Never) to
4 (Very often). The scale uses a dichotomous-scoring system such that responses to each ques-
tion are dichotomized into whether a particular symptom is present or not. For example, for
the question “How often do you have problems remembering appointments or obligations?”, a
response of “sometimes”, “often”, or “very often” indicates the presence of the symptom,
whereas a response of “never” and “rarely” indicates the absence of the symptom. The total
score was calculated by counting all positive symptoms, yielding a theoretical range of 0–18.
ADHD functional impairment questionnaire. The functional impairment questions
were selected from the Current Symptoms Scale developed by [90]. The questionnaire includes
10 questions regarding functional impairment in ADHD. The purpose of this scale is to under-
stand how ADHD symptoms potentially impact different areas of an individual’s life (i.e.,
school, work, relationships). These questions were presented immediately following the Adult
ADHD Self-report Scale. Participants were asked to indicate “To what extent do the problems
you may have selected on the previous questionnaire interfere with your ability to function in
each of these areas of life activities?” Each item has the following response options: “never/
rarely”, “sometimes”, “often”, and “very often”. These items were rated on a 0-to-3 scale. The
final score was the average score of the 10 items, with a higher score indicating greater func-
tional impairment.
Adult Dispositional Hyperfocus Questionnaire. The adult dispositional hyperfocus
questionnaire is a 12-item scale measuring one’s tendency to experience hyperfocus [13], a
state of heightened, focused attention that individuals with ADHD frequently report. Each
item has the following response options: “never”, “1–2 times every 6 months”, “1–2 times per
month”, “once a week”, “2–3 times a week”, and “daily”. These items were rated on a 1-to-6
scale (1 corresponding to “never” and 6 corresponding to “daily”). The final score was the
average rating of all items, with a higher score indicating a greater tendency to engage in
hyperfocus.
Procedure
Questionnaires were presented to the participants in a pseudo-random order such that those
of the same category were always presented together. The presentation order within each cate-
gory was randomized for each participant, except that the Functional Impairment Question-
naire was always presented immediately after the Adult ADHD Self-report Scale.
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 10 / 31
Data analysis
Our data analytic plan consisted of two main steps. First, we conducted a series of confirma-
tory factor analyses to explore the factor structure of distractibility. Second, in structural equa-
tion modeling, we related our models of distractibility to individual differences in ADHD
symptoms, functional impairments, and hyperfocus. Throughout the analyses, we used Sample
1 for exploratory purposes and Sample 2 for replication.
Data analysis was conducted in the R environment. Latent factor modeling was conducted
using the lavaan package [91] with robust maximum likelihood, which produces the Satorra-
Bentler χ
2
statistic (S-B χ
2
) for the overall model fit. A non-significant χ
2
value is often desir-
able, but with a large sample, even trivial mis-specifications can produce a significant result.
For this reason, we supplemented the χ
2
statistic with additional fit indices, such as the com-
parative fit index (CFI), the robust root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and
the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). By convention, the fit of a model is
deemed acceptable when CFI > .90, RMSEA < .08, and SRMR < .08. For RMSEA, we also
report the 90% confidence interval (90% CI) to assess how close it is to the boundary of misfit.
For model comparison, we report the S-B χ
2
difference test and the Bayes Factors (BFs)
derived from the BIC approximation [92]. Following recommendations by Raftery [93], we
consider 1 < BF <= 3 to be weak supporting evidence, 3 < BF <= 20 to be positive supporting
evidence, 20 < BF <= 150 to be strong supporting evidence, and BF > 150 to be very strong
supporting evidence. Conversely, we consider 1/3 <= BF < 1 to be weak evidence against,
1/20 <= BF < 1/3 to be positive evidence against, 1/150 <= BF < 1/20 to be strong evidence
against, and BF < 1/150 to be very strong evidence against.
Results
Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics for all measures for both Samples 1 and 2. Overall, all
measures had satisfactory reliability values and normal levels of skewness and kurtosis in both
samples. Compared to Sample 1, the standard deviations associated with Sample 2 were consis-
tently lower, suggesting that Sample 2 was more homogeneous, which is consistent with the
fact that they were college students enrolled in an introductory psychology course.
The full correlation matrix is presented in Fig 1. The correlations for Sample 1 are presented
below the diagonal, whereas those for Sample 2 are presented above the diagonal. As is clear
from Fig 1, the patterns of the correlations were generally comparable for the two samples.
Goal 1: Understanding the relationships among different facets of
distractibility
Exploratory analyses with Sample 1. The baseline measurement model consisted of
External Distraction, Thought Intrusions, Repetitive Negative Thinking, and Mind-wandering,
with each factor represented by three scales. Although this model produced a good fit
(χ
2
(48) = 157.33, p < .001; CFI = .98; RMSEA = .06 [.05, .08]; SRMR = .029; BIC = 14,948),
we modified this model in two ways based on substantive concerns [94 p.310].
First, as shown in Fig 2, the factors Thought Intrusions and Repetitive Negative Thinking
had a near-perfect correlation, and they also had similar relationships with the other factors in
the model (the model’s full structure can be found in S1 Fig). Thus, to avoid redundancy in the
model structure, we combined Thought Intrusions, Repetitive Negative Thinking into a single
factor named Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts. This move aligns with recommendations by Kline
[94], who stressed that respecification in CFA should be guided as much as possible by sub-
stantive considerations. Given the large sample sizes of the current study and the resulting
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 11 / 31
statistical power, even trivial differences may become highly significant. Here, an r = .95
means that the two factors shared about 85% to 90% of the variance (i.e., only *10% and
*15% of the unique variance left). If these highly correlated constructs were kept separate, the
meaning of a possible general factor would be highly biased (essentially adding a duplicate
indicator). Although some details differ, some previous latent-variable analysis studies have
merged some latent variables on the basis of such substantive considerations [9598].
Second, we inspected the modification indices (MI) of the model, which suggested that
allowing the residual of the TCAQ and that of the TSI to correlate with each other would pro-
duce the largest improvement in model fit (MI = 62.82). This suggestion makes sense, given
that the TCAQ and the TSI shared many similarly worded items (e.g., There are some thoughts
that enter my head without me being able to avoid it” in the former and “Some unwanted
thoughts enter my mind without me being able to do anything about it” in the latter; original
items are available at https://osf.io/8j6p4/). Thus, we added this error covariance parameter to
capture the wording similarity.
The resulting three-factor model is presented in Fig 3A. Loadings and factor correlations for
this sample (Sample 1) are indicated in bold fonts. The fit of the model was good, χ
2
(50) = 172.73,
p < .001; CFI = .98; RMSEA = .07 [.06, .08]; SRMR = .030; BIC = 14,953. The observed measures
were generally loaded strongly onto their respective factors (zs >= 28.72, ps < .001). The three
factors were also significantly correlated with each other (rs >= .53, zs >= 14.49, ps < .001). The
model also indicated a nontrivial correlation between the error terms of the TCAQ and the TSI
(r = .47, z = 9.72, p < .001). A re-inspection of the modification indices did not suggest further
alternations that would appreciably improve model fit.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics for all measures analyzed in the current study.
Scale Name Sample 1 Sample 2
mean SD skew kurtosis alpha mean SD skew kurtosis alpha
External Distraction
Imaginal Processes Inventory—Distractibility 2.30 0.92 -0.29 -0.35 0.85 2.45 0.79 -0.09 -0.26 0.78
Attentional Style Questionnaire—External 3.43 1.06 0.00 -0.10 0.81 3.58 0.99 0.03 -0.40 0.78
Attentional Control—Distraction 2.76 1.04 0.19 -0.84 0.88 3.04 0.97 0.03 -0.83 0.84
Thought Intrusions
White Bear Suppression Inventory 3.56 0.77 -0.57 0.29 0.92 3.54 0.69 -0.26 -0.15 0.90
Thought Control Ability Questionnaire 3.22 0.83 -0.15 -0.48 0.95 3.19 0.71 -0.02 -0.51 0.93
Thought Suppression Inventory 3.20 0.80 -0.05 -0.25 0.91 3.16 0.69 0.16 -0.41 0.89
Repetitive Negative Thinking
Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire 2.11 0.96 -0.22 -0.46 0.97 2.16 0.80 0.04 -0.36 0.95
Penn State Worry Questionnaire 3.37 0.98 -0.27 -0.79 0.96 3.56 0.86 -0.37 -0.55 0.95
Ruminative Response Scale—Brooding 2.39 0.78 0.19 -0.80 0.85 2.41 0.72 0.28 -0.76 0.80
Mind-Wandering
Imaginal Processes Inventory—Mind-wandering 2.15 0.95 0.03 -0.67 0.89 2.17 0.87 0.17 -0.71 0.86
Mind-wandering—Spontaneous 4.29 1.50 -0.29 -0.56 0.91 4.21 1.35 -0.03 -0.41 0.88
Daydreaming Frequency Scale 3.04 0.94 -0.04 -0.73 0.96 3.00 0.86 0.01 -0.72 0.95
ADHD-related Constructs
Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale 5.91 4.50 0.60 -0.44 0.86 7.02 4.19 0.35 -0.56 0.82
ADHD Functional Impairment 0.84 0.63 0.73 0.19 0.90 0.86 0.57 0.63 0.08 0.86
Dispositional Hyperfocus 3.44 1.10 0.01 -0.65 0.92 3.53 1.04 -0.01 -0.67 0.91
Note. SD = Standard Deviation
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215.t002
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 12 / 31
We also tested a two-factor model in which Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts and Mind-wan-
dering were combined into one factor named Internal Distraction, as well as a single-factor
model in which all three factors were combined into a single factor. In both models, the afore-
mentioned error correlation between the TCAQ and the TSI was included. However, both
models resulted in very poor model fit. Specifically, the fit of this two-factor model was appre-
ciably worse compared to the three-factor model as shown in Fig 3A, Δχ
2
(2) = 201.96,
p < .001; BF < 1/1000 (very strong evidence against). And the fit of the single-factor model
was even worse compared to the two-factor model, Δχ
2
(1) = 290.00, p < .001; BF < 1/1000
(very strong evidence against).
The results so far indicate that External Distraction, Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts, and
Mind-wandering were substantially correlated constructs. Therefore, we introduced a higher-
order common factor d to capture their shared variance, as shown in Fig 3B. We chose the let-
ter d for this general factor to echo other general factors that have been proposed in the litera-
ture, such as the general psychopathology factor p [99] and general intelligence factor g [100].
Fig 1. A correlation matrix for all measures. Sample 1: below the diagonal line. Sample 2: above the diagonal line.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215.g001
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 13 / 31
This change in model structure from correlated factors to a higher-order common factor does
not alter the model fit. The first-order loadings remained the same as in the correlational
model. The three first-order factors (External Distraction, Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts, and
Mind-wandering) were all significantly loaded onto d (zs >= 19.03, p < .001).
To quantify the magnitude of the shared variance, we calculated the coefficient omega at
Level 2 (ω
L2
), which indicates the proportion of variance among the first-order factors attribut-
able to d. We found that ω
L2
= .83, indicating that d explained 83% of the total variance in
External Distraction, Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts, and Mind-wandering. The variances in the
first-order factors that could not be explained by d were captured by their respective residual
terms (.57, .36, and .28, respectively, as shown in Fig 3B). These residual terms were all signifi-
cantly different from zero (zs >= 4.48, p < .001). Thus, although d captured a substantial
amount of shared variance, there were still some construct-specific variances left.
We then compared the strength of the second-order loadings on d. To do so, we imposed
a series of equality constraints on the second-order loadings of the model. Constraining all
three factors to load equally on d substantially worsened model fit, Δχ
2
(1) = 19.19, p < .001;
BF = 1/212.43 (very strong evidence against). Thus, the three second-order loadings were
not equal. To better specify the source of this non-equivalence of the d loadings, we con-
ducted pairwise comparisons by constraining two of the loadings to be equal at each time
and compared it to the model without the constraints. Constraining Mind-wandering and
External Distraction to load equally on d substantially worsened model fit, Δχ
2
(1) = 14.63,
p < .001; BF < 1/1000 (very strong evidence against). Constraining Unwanted Intrusive
Thoughts and External Distraction to load equally on d also substantially worsened model fit,
Δχ
2
(1) = 14.47, p < .001; BF = 1/26.19 (strong evidence against). But constraining Unwanted
Fig 2. Latent correlations among external distraction, thought intrusions, repetitive negative thinking, and mind-
wandering. The correlation between Thought Intrusions and Repetitive Negative Thinking was close to 1 in both
samples.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215.g002
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 14 / 31
Intrusive Thoughts and Mind-wandering to load equally on d did not worsen model fit,
Δχ
2
(1) = .67, p = .41; BF = 16.17 (positive evidence in favor of). These results indicate that
the loading of External Distraction on d was relatively weaker (albeit still significant) com-
pared to those of Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts and Mind-wandering.
Fig 3. Correlational and general factor models of distractibility. Fully standardized estimates are shown, with bold fonts
indicating Sample 1 and regular fonts indicating Sample 2. IPI-D: Imaginal Processes Inventory-Distractibility; ASQ-E: Attentional
Style Questionnaire-External; AC-D: Attentional Control-Distraction; PTQ: Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire; PSWQ: Penn
State Worry Questionnaire; RRS-B: Ruminative Response Scale-Brooding; WBSI: White Bear Suppression Inventory; TCAQ:
Thought Control Ability Questionnaire; TSI: Thought Suppression Inventory (revised); IPI-MW: Imaginal Processes Inventory-
Mind-wandering; MW-S: Mind-wandering-Spontaneous; DDFS: Daydreaming Frequency Scale.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215.g003
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 15 / 31
Replication with Sample 2. After the explorations of various models using Sample 1 as
described above, we tested whether the results could be replicated using Sample 2. As shown in
Fig 2, the correlation between Thought Intrusions and Repetitive Negative Thinking was again
close to 1 (r = .92) and thus the two factors should be combined. Modification indices again
suggested that adding the residual correlation between TCAQ and TSI would produce the larg-
est improvement in model fit (MI = 31.84). Thus, adding the error covariance is also justified.
The same three-factor model as shown in Fig 3A resulted in a good model fit, χ
2
(50) =
163.24, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .07 [.06, .08]; SRMR = .041; BIC = 12,526. The loadings
and factor correlations for the Sample 2 data are shown in regular font below those for the
Sample 1 data. Similar to Sample 1, the factor loadings (zs >= 24.88, ps < .001) and factor cor-
relations (rs >= .32, zs >= 7.37, ps < .001) were all significant. Furthermore, the Sample 2 data
did not support a two-factor model, Δχ
2
(2) = 296.73, p < .001; BF < 1/1000 (very strong
evidence against), or a one-factor model, Δχ
2
(1) = 893.97, p < .001; BF < 1/1000 (very strong
evidence against).
For the higher-order model as shown in Fig 3B, the three first-order factors were all signifi-
cantly loaded onto d (zs >= 8.92, p < .001). The ω
L2
of d was.77, indicating that d explained
77% of the total variance in External Distraction, Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts, and Mind-
wandering (83% in Sample 1). In addition, the residual terms of External Distraction,
Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts, and Mind-wandering were all significantly different from zero
(zs >= 2.94, p <= .003).
Comparisons of the second-order loadings once again show that External Distraction was
relatively weakly (albeit still significantly) loaded onto d. Specifically, constraining all three sec-
ond-order loadings to be equal substantially worsened model fit, Δχ
2
(1) = 47.95, p < .001;
BF < 1/1000 (very strong evidence against). Constraining Mind-wandering and External Dis-
traction to load equally on d substantially worsened model fit, Δχ
2
(1) = 40.98, p < .001;
BF < 1/1000 (very strong evidence against). Constraining Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts and
External Distraction to load equally on d substantially worsened model fit, Δχ
2
(1) = 28.03,
p < .001; BF < 1/1000 (very strong evidence against). But constraining Unwanted Intrusive
Thoughts and Mind-wandering to load equally on d did not worsen model fit, Δχ
2
(1) = 1.91,
p = .17; BF = 9.15 (positive evidence in favor of).
Overall, all of the above results based on Sample 2 were highly consistent with those based
on Sample 1.
Measurement invariance. Next, we conducted a measurement invariance analysis to eval-
uate the replicability of the model at the parameter level (i.e., beyond the global model struc-
ture). Measurement invariance analysis involves testing the equivalence of models in
independent samples to assure that the same constructs are being assessed in each sample. Spe-
cifically, a series of cross-group equality constraints are incrementally added to a multiple-
group confirmatory factor analysis model. Four levels of invariance are typically tested: (a)
configural invariance (i.e., equal model structure across samples), (b) metric invariance (i.e.,
equal loadings across samples), (c) scalar invariance (i.e., equal intercepts across samples), and
(d) residual invariance (i.e., equal residuals across samples). If the model with new constraints
does not fit appreciably worse than the model without those constraints, then the invariance
hypothesis is retained.
Currently, there is no consensus on the appropriate criteria to assess invariance. We made
our decisions based on multiple fit indices, including ΔCFI, ΔRMSEA, ΔSRMR, and Bayes
Factors. Δχ
2
tests were not recommended as a criterion to assess invariance when the sample
size is large [94]. Thus, they were reported for completeness but not considered in the assess-
ment of invariance. All models in this analysis were identified using the effect-coding
method [101].
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 16 / 31
The results, as shown in Table 3, indicate that the higher-order model passed all four levels
of invariance. This indicates that the scales measured the same factors (configural) with the
same unit of measurement (loadings), same origin (intercepts), and the same precision (resid-
uals). Note that the Bayes Factor for scalar invariance was 1/2.46, indicating weak evidence
against scalar invariance over metric invariance. However, considering that ΔCFI, ΔRMSEA,
and ΔSRMR were all well below common cut-off values [94], we retained scalar invariance. It
should be noted that metric invariance may be considered particularly important in invariance
testing because it ensures that relations among the factors and measured variables are similar
across samples. Overall, these results indicate that the higher-order general distractibility
model was highly replicable at the parameter level.
Goal 2: Linking distractibility to ADHD symptoms and hyperfocus
After establishing the higher-order model of distractibility, we proceeded with our next goal:
applying the model to understand individual differences in ADHD symptoms and hyperfocus.
Again, we used Sample 1 for exploratory analyses and Sample 2 for replication.
Exploratory analyses with Sample 1. First, we explored how well d alone could account
for variances in ADHD symptoms, ADHD functional impairments, and hyperfocus. We built
a model in which the three variables were simultaneously regressed onto d. A schematic illus-
tration of the model is given in Fig 4A. The fit of the model was good, χ
2
(83) = 320.35, p <
.001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .07 [.06, .08]; SRMR = .041; BIC = 19,528. Table 4 shows the struc-
tural paths associated with d. As shown in the table, a higher d was strongly related to a higher
level of ADHD symptomatology and greater functional impairments. Notably, d was also posi-
tively related to hyperfocus, such that a higher d (i.e., higher general distractibility) was associ-
ated with a stronger tendency to engage in hyperfocus.
Next, we explored whether any diversity aspects of distractibility could explain any addi-
tional variance. To do so, we built models in which the three variables were regressed onto the
residual variances of External Distraction, Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts, and Mind-wandering
in addition to d. These residual variances were construct-specific variances in each specific
form of distraction that were not explained by d (see Fig 3B). To use residual variance as a pre-
dictor, we created a latent factor to capture the residual variance in each group factor following
instructions from [102]. These models are shown in Fig 4B–4D. The structural paths associated
with the residual factor, therefore, test the explanatory power of construct-specific variance
that is unrelated to d. Note that this procedure had to be conducted separately for each first-
level factor because regressing on all residuals simultaneously leads to non-identification issues
Table 3. Measurement invariance analysis of the general distractibility model.
Model Comparison χ
2
df Δχ
2
CFI RMSEA [90% CI] SRMR BF
1. configural invariance 336.18 100 .98 .07 [.06, .07] .033
2. metric invariance 2 vs. 1 366.11 111 28.53** .97 .07 [.06, .07] .045 > 1000
3. scalar invariance 3 vs. 2 446.15 122 91.76*** .97 .07 [.06, .08] .050 1/2.46
4. residual invariance 4 vs. 3 474.52 138 28.75* .97 .07 [.06, .07] .050 > 1000
Note. The Δχ
2
column shows the Satorra-Bentler-adjusted Δχ
2
difference test, which is a function of two standard (not robust) χ
2
statistics.
* p < .05,
** p < .01,
*** p < .001.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215.t003
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 17 / 31
due to linear dependency (for a technical discussion of the issue, see Zhang et al. [103, p.536];
for a previous example of separately testing the residuals, see Berkowitz and Stern [104]).
The model shown in Fig 4B used the residual of External Distraction as an additional pre-
dictor. The model had a good fit, χ
2
(80) = 302.00, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .07 [.06, .08];
SRMR = .036; BIC = 19,527. The key results are summarized in Table 4. External Distraction
residual was not significantly associated with ADHD symptoms or functional impairments. A
negative relationship emerged between the residual of External Distraction and hyperfocus,
such that a higher susceptibility to external distraction was associated with a weaker hyperfo-
cus tendency after controlling for d.
The model shown in Fig 4C used the residual of Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts as an addi-
tional predictor. The model had a good fit, χ
2
(80) = 316.49, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .07
[.06, .08]; SRMR = .040; BIC = 19,542. The results, summarized in Table 4, show that
Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts residual did not significantly explain additional variance in any
of the variables.
Fig 4. Schematic illustrations of models testing how the unity and diversity aspects of distractibility were related to ADHD
symptoms, functional ADHD impairments, and hyperfocus. Panel A shows a model that tests how well d alone could account for
variances in ADHD symptoms, functional impairments, and hyperfocus. The models in panels B-D test whether External
Distraction, Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts, and Mind-wandering explain additional variance over and above d. Specifically, in
Panel B, d and the non-d residual of External Distraction served as predictors. In Panel C, d and the non-d residual of Unwanted
Intrusive Thoughts served as predictors. In Panel D, d and the non-d residual of Mind-wandering served as predictors. In all panels,
manifest variables were not shown. The directional paths were not meant to imply causation.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215.g004
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 18 / 31
The model shown in Fig 4D used the residual of Mind-wandering as an additional predictor.
The fit of the model was good, χ
2
(80) = 316.06, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .07 [.06, .08];
SRMR = .040; BIC = 19,539. The results, summarized in Table 4, show that Mind-wandering
residual was not significantly associated with ADHD symptoms or functional impairments.
There was a positive relationship with hyperfocus, such that a higher susceptibility to mind-
wandering was associated with a stronger tendency to engage in hyperfocus even after control-
ling for d.
Overall, our analyses using Sample 1 show that d was the dominant predictor of ADHD
symptoms and functional impairments. When the residual factors were added, there were
some minor fluctuations in the effects of d, but d still remained a highly significant predictor.
In contrast, the residuals of External Distraction, Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts, and Mind-
wandering did not explain meaningful variance in ADHD symptoms and functional
impairments.
The results for hyperfocus were more complex. While d was also positively associated with
hyperfocus, the residuals of External Distraction and Mind-wandering also explained addi-
tional variance. Interestingly, when controlling for d, increased susceptibility to external dis-
traction was associated with a weaker tendency to engage in hyperfocus, whereas increased
susceptibility to mind-wandering was associated with a stronger tendency to engage in hyper-
focus. This contrasting pattern of results will be discussed in more depth later in the Discus-
sion section.
Replication with Sample 2. All the results for Sample 1 were replicated in the Sample 2
data (see Table 4). First, the model with d as the only predictor (Fig 4A) had a good fit, χ
2
(83)
= 258.75, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .06 [.06, .07]; SRMR = .046; BIC = 16,639. A higher d
was associated with a higher level of ADHD symptomatology, greater functional impairments,
and a stronger hyperfocus tendency.
Second, the model with the residual of External Distraction as an additional predictor (Fig
4B) had a good fit, χ
2
(80) = 242.69, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .06 [.05, .07]; SRMR = .042;
Table 4. Standardized path coefficients of the unity and diversity components of distractibility predicting ADHD symptoms, ADHD functional impairments, and
hyperfocus.
Models Sample 1 Sample 2
ADHD Symptoms Functional Impairments Hyperfocus ADHD Symptoms Functional Impairments Hyperfocus
Model A
d .78*** .66*** .36*** .74*** .67*** .37***
Model B
d .78*** .66*** .39*** .74*** .68*** .40***
External Distraction Residual -.01 -.03 -.19*** .01 -.06 -.16***
Model C
d .80*** .65*** .35*** .74*** .65*** .38***
Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts
Residual
-.07 .01 .04 -.02 .06 -.02
Model D
d .75*** .65*** .31*** .72*** .67*** .29***
Mind-wandering Residual .11 .04 .16* .08 .02 .22*
Note. The results of models A-D correspond to the models shown in Fig 4.
*p < .05,
***p < .001.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215.t004
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 19 / 31
BIC = 16,641. External Distraction residual was not significantly associated with ADHD symp-
toms and functional impairments, but it was negatively and significantly associated with
hyperfocus.
Third, the model with the residual of Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts as an additional predic-
tor (Fig 4C) had a good fit, χ
2
(80) = 255.23, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .07 [.06, .07];
SRMR = .046; BIC = 16,654. Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts residual did not significantly
explain additional variance in any of the variables.
Finally, the model with the residual of Mind-wandering as an additional predictor (Fig 4D)
also had a good fit, χ
2
(80) = 252.46, p < .001; CFI = .97; RMSEA = .07 [.06, .07]; SRMR = .044;
BIC = 16,651. Mind-wandering residual was not significantly associated with ADHD symp-
toms and functional impairments, but it was positively and significantly associated with
hyperfocus.
Additional analyses. We also conducted two additional analyses, both of which can be
found in S2 File. First, the relationship between d and hyperfocus was relatively weaker com-
pared to ADHD symptoms and functional impairments (Supplemental Analysis 1 in S2 File).
Second, while d was substantially associated with both inattention symptoms (e.g., having diffi-
culty keeping attention) and hyperactive/impulsive symptoms (e.g., fidgeting or squirming
with hands or feet), it was more strongly associated with inattention symptoms (Supplemental
Analysis 2 in S2 File). None of the residual factors explained meaningful variance. Both analy-
ses were replicated in Sample 2.
Discussion
The current study aimed to understand the factor structure of distractibility. Using a latent
modeling approach, we assessed how individual differences in self-reported vulnerability to
several forms of real-world distraction were related. Based on the pattern of the correlations,
we developed a latent-variable model that incorporates a higher-order common factor to cap-
ture individual differences in general distractibility. We then examined how this higher-order
model can account for individual differences in ADHD behavioral symptoms, ADHD func-
tional impairments, and hyperfocus.
With two large samples, we conducted exploratory analyses using the first sample and an
internal replication using the second sample. It is worth noting that the characteristics of the
two samples were substantially different in terms of age (26.9 ± 5.0 in Sample 1 vs. 18.8 ± 1.2 in
Sample 2), gender distribution (45.3% female in Sample 1 vs. 66.3% female in Sample 2), racial
composition (67.7% white in Sample 1 vs. 56.8% white in Sample 2), and occupation (33.9%
college students vs. 100% college students). Despite these differences, all key results in Sample
1 were successfully replicated in Sample 2, thus providing solid empirical ground for the fol-
lowing discussion of results.
Goal 1: Understanding the relationships among different facets of
distractibility
Whereas the term “distraction” is commonly used to describe how our attention is diverted by
both internal (e.g., our own thoughts) and external (e.g., pop-up advertisements) sources in
daily life, the study of distraction has focused heavily on the latter. Although other fields have
studied phenomena such as mind-wandering and intrusive thoughts, there has been relatively
little integration between them until recently [55].
Extending previous findings, a critical finding of our study is the identification of a higher-
order factor that could be construed to represent a general distractibility trait. Indeed, we
found that there was very much in common among external distraction, unwanted intrusive
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 20 / 31
thoughts, and mind-wandering: d accounted for about 80% of the total variance in the three
factors (83% in Sample 1 and 77% in Sample 2). A measurement invariance analysis further
showed generalizability of the higher-order model at the parameter level. Taken together, our
findings underscore the notable shared variance among several distractibility-related con-
structs, suggesting a reliable trait reflecting individual differences in (perceived) distractibility.
Goal 2: Linking distractibility to ADHD symptoms and hyperfocus
ADHD symptoms and associated functional impairments. In Goal 2 of the current
study, we related our higher-order model of distractibility to individual differences in self-
reported ADHD symptoms and functional impairments. Across two samples, we found that d
was substantially related to individual differences in ADHD symptoms and functional impair-
ments. Furthermore, d was more related to the inattentive symptoms than to the hyperactive/
impulsive symptoms (see Supplemental Analysis 2 in S2 File). These results further speak to
the validity of d as a measure of general distractibility.
These results also have important implications for the nature of the heightened distractibil-
ity in ADHD. Existing research on distractibility and ADHD has often examined specific facets
of distractibility in isolation and thereby neglected the substantial overlap among different fac-
ets of distractibility. When ADHD is simultaneously associated with vulnerability to multiple
forms of distraction, it may be more parsimonious to view these associations as reflecting a sin-
gle association with general distractibility rather than separate associations with each specific
form of distractibility. In fact, our results show that once d was included in the model, none of
the residual factors explained variance in ADHD symptoms above and beyond d. In particular,
External Distraction did not explain any meaningful variance in ADHD symptoms and func-
tional impairments beyond d despite its substantial construct-specific variance. Thus, the
results indicate that the heightened distractibility in ADHD symptomatology is best character-
ized by general distractibility instead of by any single type of distraction alone.
Hyperfocus. Our findings provide insight into the nature of hyperfocus. We discovered
that individuals who were more generally distractible tended to report more frequent hyperfo-
cus episodes. This counterintuitive relationship implies that focused and distracted attention
might share some underlying features. One possible interpretation is that individuals often
hyperfocus on the same subjects that distract them from more critical tasks. For example,
someone might hyperfocus on watching TV, which serves as a distraction from their work
assignments. Alternatively, distractibility and hyperfocus might both be indicative of deficits
in attentional control. Consequently, individuals who are easily distracted by irrelevant infor-
mation may also tend to hyperfocus on certain subjects because they have difficulties control-
ling when they should focus attention and when they should disengage attention.
Interestingly, hyperfocus was explained by other predictors besides d. When d was con-
trolled for, hyperfocus was positively related to mind-wandering but was negatively related to
external distraction (see Table 4). What might explain this contrasting pair of relationships?
Current theories of mind-wandering consider perceptual decoupling, or reduced process-
ing of immediate perceptual input, as a unique feature of mind-wandering [35, 105]. Both
behavioral and neurological evidence indicates that mind-wandering is associated with
reduced processing of external information and events [35]. It has been suggested that percep-
tual decoupling is critical for mind-wandering to unfold because it protects the internal train
of thought from external disruptions [12]. Similarly, many descriptions of hyperfocus experi-
ences include common qualities such as distorted time perception, failure to attend to the
world, ignoring personal needs, feelings of total engrossment, etc. [13]. Therefore, it seems
plausible that a decoupling process may be involved in hyperfocus to reduce the processing of
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 21 / 31
information that is not related to the current hyperfocus subject. This potential similarity
between hyperfocus and mind-wandering may also explain why external distraction was nega-
tively related to hyperfocus once d was accounted for: those who are more sensitive to sensory
information may have more difficulties immersing themselves in a hyperfocus state.
In sum, whereas ADHD symptoms mostly reflect heightened general distractibility, hyper-
focus appears to be a more complex construct that displays qualities of both distraction (in
terms of its association with d) and concentration (in terms of decoupling oneself from sub-
jects unrelated to the hyperfocus content). These results suggest the utility of considering
diversity aspects of distractibility in explaining other variables of interest.
Limitations and future directions
As the first large-scale latent-variable study of its kind, the current study goes substantially
beyond previous studies reviewed earlier [54, 55, 57] that tried to make a case for a general dis-
tractibility factor. Moreover, the current study also related the d factor to two ADHD-related
variables (ADHD symptoms and hyperfocus). Despite these substantial contributions to the
literature, the study is also limited in a number of ways. We will discuss two in detail here.
Alternative interpretations of what d represents. One limitation of the current study is
that, although the results reported above demonstrate that it is possible to statistically extract a
general d factor, a latent-variable analysis like the one we conducted, by itself, does not specify
what the d factor really represents as a psychological construct. As such, the current study
leaves open the possibility that the d factor can be accounted for—at least in part, if not
entirely—by other psychological constructs. Note that this interpretational ambiguity of a gen-
eral factor is not unique to the current study; in fact, it is also applicable to other latent-variable
studies postulating a general factor, such as the g factor for intelligence [100], the p factor for
psychopathology [99], and the Common EF factor for executive functions [47, 48].
Nevertheless, two alternative interpretations–one methodological and one psychological–
need to be considered in the context of the current study.
One such possibility is that the d factor might be a methodological artefact in that most of
its variance can be explained by substantial method variance attributable to response biases of
some sort, such as social desirability [106] or a tendency to complain about cognition [107].
Given that the current study focused on a large number of self-report measures, we cannot
completely rule out such a possibility. Nevertheless, we suspect that response biases, by them-
selves, are unlikely to be able to account for substantial shared variance among the measures
administered in the current study for a number of reasons. First, the questionnaires used in
the current study are well-validated, with many of them being used extensively in both
research and clinical settings. Second, the current study was conducted online, with partici-
pants completing the questionnaires anonymously, without facing any experimenter in per-
son, which presumably reduces the likelihood that participants’ responses are strongly affected
by at least one such potential bias-related factor, social desirability. Finally, it is difficult to test
a response-bias account of the d factor, given that it is not necessarily clear what specific
response biases might be operating that are strong enough to affect participants’ responses to
all of the questionnaire measures. Thus, it is unlikely that the d factor is a pure methodological
artefact reflecting unspecified method variance of some sort.
A second alternative interpretation that needs to be considered is that the d factor can be
substantially explained by some other psychological constructs. One interesting possibility is
that the d factor can be substantially (if not entirely) accounted for by a Big Five personality
trait, neuroticism [108]. Given that neuroticism is associated with one’s tendency to experience
negative affects, such as anger, anxiety, and depression [109], it is likely that individual
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 22 / 31
differences in neuroticism substantially overlaps with the distractibility constructs examined
in the current study that are clearly linked to negative affect (i.e., the unwanted intrusive
thoughts factor that encompasses the repetitive negative thinking and intrusive thoughts fac-
tors). What is not clear is to what extent neuroticism is related to the other two distractibility
constructs less directly associated with negative affect, external distraction, and mind-wander-
ing. As shown in the higher-order common factor model (Fig 3B), however, the d-factor load-
ings are not necessarily dominated by the unwanted intrusive thoughts factor. Moreover, it is
generally the case that the magnitudes of correlations with neuroticism are substantially higher
for repetitive negative thinking and intrusive thoughts [110112] than for mind-wandering
and external distraction [113117]. Thus, neuroticism, conceptualized as a psychological trait,
may not be able to fully explain the d factor.
Because we did not include a measure of neuroticism in the current study, it is not possible
to precisely specify how much of the variance in the d factor can be accounted for by neuroti-
cism. However, just as Friedman et al. [118] were able to demonstrate that general intelligence
or IQ accounted for only half (*50%) of the variation in a general executive function factor
(or Common EF), future studies should be able to quantify the extent to which neuroticism
and other general factors can explain the variance in d.
More broadly, the effort to characterize what the d factor represents psychologically will
likely be facilitated by systematic attempts to examine the relationship between d and other
psychological constructs that tap into attention and cognitive control, such as working mem-
ory capacity, general fluid intelligence, executive function, and attentional control, given that
all these constructs have been theorized to play an overarching role in maintaining task focus
and resisting distraction interference [48, 49, 51, 119, 120]. Also, given the prominent role of
distractibility in various psychiatric disorders, it might be worthwhile examining the relation-
ship between d and the general psychopathology factor p [99].
Generalizability of the current results to behavioral measures of distractibility. As a
first step toward achieving a comprehensive understanding of the relationships among differ-
ent facets of distractibility, the present study conducted a latent-variable analysis focusing on
self-report questionnaires only. Given a strong need for a comprehensive assessment of several
distractibility-related constructs simultaneously in a methodologically rigorous way (e.g.,
latent-variable analysis), we believe that our focus on self-report measures in this study is fully
justified, especially in the light of the state of the literature on the notion of a general distracti-
bility factor [54, 55, 57].
Having said that, however, we are also fully aware that there are other ways of assessing dif-
ferent facets of distractibility, such as behavioral tasks, experience-sampling in the laboratory
as well as in more naturalistic everyday settings, and even cognitive neuroscience measures
(e.g., EEG/ERP, neuroimaging). Thus, another important limitation of the current study is
that we did not include any such non-questionnaire-based ways of assessing different facets of
distractibility that would have helped us evaluate whether our findings (especially regarding
the d factor) can be generalized to other assessment methods. Given that two of the distractibil-
ity-related constructs examined here–external distraction and mind-wandering–have been
studied with multiple methods, relating the current results to those based on those other
assessment methods (especially behavioral tasks) seems to be an important and necessary next
step. In this regard, we mention two important considerations here.
First, the current state of the field is such that the availability of well-established objective
behavioral measures (e.g., based on RTs or error rates) is limited, especially for more clinically
oriented constructs like intrusive thoughts and repetitive negative thinking. Even when there
are some behavioral tasks available, their psychometric properties have been found suboptimal
[121]. For example, Meier [56] demonstrated that behavioral measures of external distraction
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 23 / 31
developed earlier [54, 55] had an internal consistency of.08—.26, which seems insufficient to
reliably capture individual differences in external distractibility. Moreover, some behavioral
measures of one’s ability to filter out external distractions (e.g., flanker and antisaccade) do not
correlate well with each other, leading some researchers to suggest that individual differences
in interference-control abilities as assessed by these measures are task-specific [122]. Thus, it
currently seems difficult to conduct a large-scale latent-variable study of the sort reported here
that examines multiple distractibility-related constructs simultaneously using multiple mea-
sures for each construct not only for questionnaires but also for behavioral tasks. Further task
development and validation seem necessary before such a study can be conducted in a rigor-
ous, psychometrically sound manner.
Second, it has become increasingly clear that different ways of assessing the same distracti-
bility-related construct (e.g., mind-wandering) may not necessarily correlate substantially with
each other [123, 124]. For example, Kane et al. [124, p.1278] recently demonstrated that mind-
wandering, as measured by thought probes in laboratory settings, is not correlated with mind-
wandering in real life, as measured with experience-sampling, leading them to conclude that
“any relation between laboratory and overall daily-life mind-wandering propensities is not
robust”.
More generally, the relationship (or lack thereof) between questionnaires and behavioral
measures has become an important issue that goes beyond the measurement of distractibility
[117, 125], especially in two research domains closely related to distractibility: executive func-
tions [126] and self-control [75]. Thus, it is quite possible that the current results based on
questionnaires may not generalize to those based on behavioral measures.
We should hasten to emphasize here, however, that the potentially limited generalizabil-
ity of the questionnaire-based results to other assessment methods does not negate the sig-
nificance of the current results. In fact, an emerging view is that self-report ratings and
behavioral tasks may both capture important but different aspects of the same construct,
making them complementary to each other [75, 76, 127]. For example, according to a recent
framework [127], the d factor, as measured by questionnaires, may capture individual differ-
ences in the control of distraction that (a) is involved in everyday life situations (e.g., attend-
ing lectures), (b) is often associated with emotional content (e.g., controlling worrying
thoughts), (c) reflects metacognitive awareness (raters needed to have some insight into
their abilities), and (d) involves strategic control over the environments (e.g., one might
intentionally muting cellphones while working to avoid distractions). These aspects of dis-
traction control are not captured by laboratory-based behavioral measures that are associ-
ated with clear, well-defined task goals, but are emotionally neutral and devoid of rich
everyday contexts.
Overall, an important future challenge toward achieving a comprehensive understanding of
the nature and structure of individual differences in distractibility is not only to consider mul-
tiple facets of distractibility simultaneously but also to develop a unified framework that can
synthesize potentially different patterns of results we may see for different ways of assessing
distractibility.
Supporting information
S1 Fig. The structure of the baseline measurement model.
(PNG)
S1 File. Summary of fit indices.
(PDF)
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 24 / 31
S2 File. Additional analyses on ADHD symptoms and hyperfocus.
(PDF)
Acknowledgments
We thank Madison Carlson for assistance in IRB application and data collection for this
project.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Han Zhang, Akira Miyake, John Jonides.
Formal analysis: Han Zhang.
Funding acquisition: John Jonides.
Methodology: Akira Miyake.
Resources: John Jonides.
Supervision: Akira Miyake, Priti Shah, John Jonides.
Writing – original draft: Han Zhang.
Writing – review & editing: Akira Miyake, Jahla Osborne, Priti Shah, John Jonides.
References
1. James W. Attention. In: The Principles of Psychology. Macmillan; 1890.
2. Wallace JC, Vodanovich SJ. Workplace safety performance: Conscientiousness, cognitive failure, and
their interaction. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology. 2003-10; 8(4):316–327. https://doi.org/
10.1037/1076-8998.8.4.316 PMID: 14570526
3. Wammes JD, Seli P, Cheyne JA, Boucher PO, Smilek D. Mind wandering during lectures II: Relation
to academic performance. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology. 2016-03; 2(1):33–48.
https://doi.org/10.1037/stl0000055
4. Hansen CP. A causal model of the relationship among accidents, biodata, personality, and cognitive
factors. Journal of Applied Psychology. 1989-02; 74(1):81–90. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.74.
1.81 PMID: 2925559
5. Eysenck MW, Graydon J. Susceptibility to distraction as a function of personality. Personality and Indi-
vidual Differences. 1989-01-01; 10(6):681–687. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(89)90227-4
6. Harvey P, Winters KC, Weintraub S, Neale JM. Distractibility in children vulnerable to psychopathol-
ogy. Journal of abnormal psychology. 1981; 90(4):298. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.90.4.298
PMID: 7264059
7. Kessler R, Adler L, Barkley R, Biederman J, Conners K, Demler O, et al. The prevalence and corre-
lates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.
American Journal of psychiatry. 2006; 163(4):716–723. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716
PMID: 16585449
8. Lepisto
¨
T, Soininen M, Čeponien R, Almqvist F, Na
¨
a
¨
ta
¨
nen R, Aronen ET. Auditory event-related poten-
tial indices of increased distractibility in children with major depression. Clinical Neurophysiology.
2004-03-01; 115(3):620–627. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2003.10.020 PMID: 15036058
9. Marsh JE, Vachon F, So
¨
rqvist P. Increased Distractibility in Schizotypy: Independent of Individual Dif-
ferences in Working Memory Capacity? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2017-03; 70
(3):565–578. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1172094 PMID: 27028661
10. Rachman S. Part I. Unwanted intrusive cognitions. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy.
1981; 3(3):89–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/0146-6402(81)90007-2
11. Watkins ER. Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin. 2008; 134
(2):163. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163 PMID: 18298268
12. Smallwood J, Schooler JW. The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin. 2006-11; 132(6):946–958.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946 PMID: 17073528
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 25 / 31
13. Hupfeld KE, Abagis TR, Shah P. Living “in the zone”: hyperfocus in adult ADHD. ADHD Attention Defi-
cit and Hyperactivity Disorders. 2019-06; 11(2):191–208. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12402-019-00296-6
PMID: 30267329
14. Dreisbach G, Goschke T. How positive affect modulates cognitive control: reduced perseveration at
the cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cog-
nition. 2004; 30(2):343. PMID: 14979809
15. Forster S, Robertson DJ, Jennings A, Asherson P, Lavie N. Plugging the attention deficit: Perceptual
load counters increased distraction in ADHD. Neuropsychology. 2013; 28(1):91. https://doi.org/10.
1037/neu0000020 PMID: 24219607
16. Kim SY, Kim MS, Chun MM. Concurrent working memory load can reduce distraction. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences. 2005; 102(45):16524–16529. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.
0505454102 PMID: 16258067
17. Dornic S, Fernaeus SE. Individual differences in high-load tasks: The effect of verbal distraction.
Department of Psychology, University of Stockholm; 1981.
18. Easterbrook JA. The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychologi-
cal Review. 1959; 66(3):183–201. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0047707 PMID: 13658305
19. Alting T, Markham R. Test anxiety and distractibility. Journal of Research in Personality. 1993-06-01;
27(2):134–137. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1993.1009
20. Derryberry D, Reed MA. Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2002; p. 225–236. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.111.2.225
PMID: 12003445
21. Lyubomirsky S, Kasri F, Zehm K. Dysphoric Rumination Impairs Concentration on Academic Tasks.
Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2003; p. 22.
22. Smallwood J. Mind-wandering While Reading: Attentional Decoupling, Mindless Reading and the Cas-
cade Model of Inattention: Mind-wandering While Reading. Language and Linguistics Compass. 2011-
02; 5(2):63–77. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00263.x
23. Stefanopoulou E, Hirsch CR, Hayes S, Adlam A, Coker S. Are attentional control resources reduced
by worry in generalized anxiety disorder? Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2014; 123(2):330. https://
doi.org/10.1037/a0036343 PMID: 24886007
24. Clark DA, Rhyno S. Unwanted Intrusive Thoughts in Nonclinical Individuals: Implications for Clinical
Disorders. In: Intrusive thoughts in clinical disorders: Theory, research, and treatment. Guilford Press;
2005. p. 1–29.
25. Sarason IG, Pierce GR, Sarason BR. Domains of Cognitive Interference. In: Cognitive Interference.
Routledge; 1996. p. 139–153.
26. Ehring T, Watkins E. Repetitive Negative Thinking as a Transdiagnostic Process. International Journal
of Cognitive Therapy—INT J COGN THER. 2008-09-01; 1:192–205. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2008.
1.3.192
27. Nolen-Hoeksema S. The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive
symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000; p. 504–511. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.
109.3.504 PMID: 11016119
28. Borkovec T, Ray WJ, Stober J. Worry: A cognitive phenomenon intimately linked to affective, physio-
logical, and interpersonal behavioral processes. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1998; 22(6):561–
576. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018790003416
29. Gustavson DE, du Pont A, Whisman MA, Miyake A. Evidence for Transdiagnostic Repetitive Negative
Thinking and Its Association with Rumination, Worry, and Depression and Anxiety Symptoms: A Com-
monality Analysis. Collabra: Psychology. 2018-01-01; 4(1):13. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.128
PMID: 30761388
30. Hallion LS, Steinman SA, Kusmierski SN. Difficulty concentrating in generalized anxiety disorder: An
evaluation of incremental utility and relationship to worry. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2018-01-01;
53:39–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2017.10.007 PMID: 29175616
31. Christoff K, Irving ZC, Fox KCR, Spreng RN, Andrews-Hanna JR. Mind-wandering as spontaneous
thought: a dynamic framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2016-11; 17(11):718–731. https://doi.
org/10.1038/nrn.2016.113 PMID: 27654862
32. Mooneyham BW, Schooler JW. The costs and benefits of mind-wandering: A review. Canadian Jour-
nal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expe
´
rimentale. 2013; 67(1):11–18.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031569 PMID: 23458547
33. Smallwood J, Andrews-Hanna J. Not all minds that wander are lost: the importance of a balanced per-
spective on the mind-wandering state. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013; 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.
2013.00441 PMID: 23966961
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 26 / 31
34. Watkins ER. Level of construal, mind wandering, and repetitive thought: Reply to McVay and Kane
(2010). Psychological Bulletin. 2010; 136(2):198. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018563
35. Schooler JW, Smallwood J, Christoff K, Handy TC, Reichle ED, Sayette MA. Meta-awareness, percep-
tual decoupling and the wandering mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011; 15(7):319–326. PMID:
21684189
36. Salkovskis PM. Cognitive-behavioural factors and the persistence of intrusive thoughts in obsessional
problems. Behaviour research and therapy. 1989; 27(6):677–682. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967
(89)90152-6 PMID: 2610662
37. Kelley TL. Interpretation of educational measurements. Interpretation of educational measurements.
World Book Co.; 1927.
38. Thorndike EL. An introduction to the theory of mental and social measurements. Teacher’s College,
Columbia University; 1913.
39. Unsworth N, McMillan BD, Brewer GA, Spillers GJ. Everyday attention failures: An individual differ-
ences investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2012; 38
(6):1765–1772. PMID: 22468805
40. Unsworth N, McMillan BD. Similarities and differences between mind-wandering and external dis-
traction: A latent variable analysis of lapses of attention and their relation to cognitive abilities. Acta
Psychologica. 2014-07; 150:14–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.04.001 PMID:
24793128
41. Biederman J, Fitzgerald M, Uchida M, Spencer TJ, Fried R, Wicks J, et al. Towards operationalising
internal distractibility (Mind Wandering) in adults with ADHD. Acta neuropsychiatrica. 2017; 29
(6):330–336. https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2016.70 PMID: 29151406
42. Chun MM, Golomb JD, Turk-Browne NB. A Taxonomy of External and Internal Attention. Annual
Review of Psychology. 2011-01-10; 62(1):73–101. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.
100427 PMID: 19575619
43. Lanier J, Noyes E, Biederman J. Mind Wandering (Internal Distractibility) in ADHD: A Literature
Review. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2021-04-01; 25(6):885–890. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1087054719865781 PMID: 31364436
44. Yee PL, Vaughan J. Integrating cognitive, personality, and social approaches to cognitive interference
and distractibility. In: Cognitive interference: Theories, methods, and findings. Routledge; 1996. p.
77–97.
45. Brosowsky NP, Smith AC, Smilek D, Seli P. On the relation between mind wandering, PTSD sympto-
mology, and self-control. Consciousness and Cognition. 2022-03-01; 99:103288. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.concog.2022.103288 PMID: 35151968
46. Seli P, Risko EF, Purdon C, Smilek D. Intrusive thoughts: linking spontaneous mind wandering and
OCD symptomatology. Psychological Research. 2017-03; 81(2):392–398. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s00426-016-0756-3 PMID: 26886575
47. Friedman NP, Miyake A. Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window
on cognitive structure. Cortex. 2017-01; 86:186–204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023
PMID: 27251123
48. Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions:
Four general conclusions. Current directions in psychological science. 2012; 21(1):8–14. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0963721411429458 PMID: 22773897
49. Burgoyne AP, Mashburn CA, Tsukahara JS, Engle RW. Attention control and process overlap theory:
Searching for cognitive processes underpinning the positive manifold. Intelligence. 2022-03-01;
91:101629. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2022.101629
50. McVay JC, Kane MJ. Conducting the train of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect,
and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition. 2009-01; 35(1):196–204. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014104 PMID:
19210090
51. McVay JC, Kane MJ. Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment
on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin. 2010-03; 136(2):188–
197. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018298 PMID: 20192557
52. McVay JC, Kane MJ. Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension?
On the influence of mind wandering and executive attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General. 2012-05; 141(2):302–320. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025250 PMID: 21875246
53. Unsworth N, McMillan BD. Mind wandering and reading comprehension: Examining the roles of work-
ing memory capacity, interest, motivation, and topic experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2013-05; 39(3):832–842. PMID: 22905931
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 27 / 31
54. Forster S, Lavie N. Distracted by your mind? Individual differences in distractibility predict mind wan-
dering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 2014-01; 40(1):251–
260. PMID: 23957365
55. Forster S, Lavie N. Establishing the Attention-Distractibility Trait. Psychological Science. 2016-02; 27
(2):203–212. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615617761 PMID: 26667659
56. Meier ME. Testing the attention-distractibility trait. Memory & Cognition. 2021-03-11. https://doi.org/
10.3758/s13421-021-01162-5 PMID: 33709382
57. Hobbiss MH, Fairnie J, Jafari K, Lavie N. Attention, mindwandering, and mood. Consciousness and
Cognition. 2019-07; 72:1–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2019.04.007 PMID: 31059975
58. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed.
American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
59. Levy F, Hay DA, McSTEPHEN M, Wood C, Waldman I. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a cate-
gory or a continuum? Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Academy
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 1997; 36(6):737–744. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-
199706000-00009 PMID: 9183127
60. Lubke GH, Hudziak JJ, Derks EM, van Bijsterveldt TC, Boomsma DI. Maternal ratings of attention
problems in ADHD: evidence for the existence of a continuum. Journal of the American Academy of
Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2009; 48(11):1085–1093. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.
0b013e3181ba3dbb PMID: 19797980
61. Gjervan B, Torgersen T, Nordahl HM, Rasmussen K. Functional Impairment and Occupational Out-
come in Adults With ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. 2012-10-01; 16(7):544–552. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1087054711413074 PMID: 21725028
62. Overbey GA, Snell WE Jr, Callis KE. Subclinical ADHD, stress, and coping in romantic relationships of
university students. Journal of attention Disorders. 2011; 15(1):67–78. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1087054709347257 PMID: 19815698
63. Sibley MH, Evans SW, Serpell ZN. Social Cognition and Interpersonal Impairment in Young Adoles-
cents with ADHD. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2010-06; 32(2):193–202.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-009-9152-2
64. Adams R, Finn P, Moes E, Flannery K, Rizzo AS. Distractibility in attention/deficit/hyperactivity disor-
der (ADHD): The virtual reality classroom. Child neuropsychology. 2009; 15(2):120–135. https://doi.
org/10.1080/09297040802169077 PMID: 18608217
65. Gumenyuk V, Korzyukov O, Escera C, Ha
¨
ma
¨
la
¨
inen M, Huotilainen M, Ha
¨
yrinen T, et al. Electrophysio-
logical evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHD children. Neuroscience letters. 2005; 374(3):212–
217. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2004.10.081 PMID: 15663965
66. Schneidt A, Jusyte A, Rauss K, Scho
¨
nenberg M. Distraction by salient stimuli in adults with attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence for the role of task difficulty in bottom-up and top-down pro-
cessing. Cortex. 2018; 101:206–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2018.01.021 PMID: 29518704
67. Abramovitch A, Schweiger A. Unwanted intrusive and worrisome thoughts in adults with Attention Defi-
cit\Hyperactivity Disorder. Psychiatry Research. 2009-08-15; 168(3):230–233. https://doi.org/10.1016/
j.psychres.2008.06.004 PMID: 19570581
68. Seli P, Smallwood J, Cheyne JA, Smilek D. On the relation of mind wandering and ADHD symptom-
atology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2015-06; 22(3):629–636. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-
014-0793-0 PMID: 25561417
69. Conner ML. Attention Deficit Disorder In Children And Adults: Strategies For Experiential Educators.
In: Experiential Education: A Critical Resource for the 21st Century. Proceedings Manual of the Annual
International Conference of the Association for Experiential Education (22nd, Austin, TX, November 3-
6, 1994); 1994. p. 7.
70. Ozel-Kizil ET, Kokurcan A, Aksoy UM, Kanat BB, Sakarya D, Bastug G, et al. Hyperfocusing as a
dimension of adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Research in Developmental Disabilities.
2016-12; 59:351–358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2016.09.016 PMID: 27681531
71. Flippin R. Hyperfocus: The ADHD Phenomenon of Intense Fixation; 2007-10-06. Available from:
https://www.additudemag.com/understanding-adhd-hyperfocus/.
72. Low K. The Relationship Between Hyperfocus and ADHD; 2021. Available from: https://www.
verywellmind.com/hyperfocus-and-add-20464.
73. Nerenberg J, Science of US. Hyperfocus: The other side of adult ADHD; 2016-07-15. Available from:
https://www.cnn.com/2016/07/15/health/adult-adhd-hyperfocus/index.html.
74. Duckworth AL, Kern ML. A meta-analysis of the convergent validity of self-control measures. Journal
of research in personality. 2011; 45(3):259–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.02.004 PMID:
21643479
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 28 / 31
75. Eisenberg IW, Bissett PG, Zeynep Enkavi A, Li J, MacKinnon DP, Marsch LA, et al. Uncovering the
structure of self-regulation through data-driven ontology discovery. Nature Communications. 2019-12;
10(1):2319. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-10301-1 PMID: 31127115
76. Saunders B, Milyavskaya M, Etz A, Randles D, Inzlicht M. Reported self-control is not meaningfully
associated with inhibition-related executive function: A bayesian analysis. Collabra: Psychology.
2018-11-20; 4(1):39. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.134
77. Simmons JP, Nelson LD, Simonsohn U. A 21 Word Solution. Social Science Research Network; 2012-
10-14. 2160588. Available from: http://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=2160588.
78. Singer JL, Antrobus JS. A factor-analytic study of daydreaming and conceptually-related cognitive and
personality variables. Perceptual and motor skills. 1963; 17(1):187–209. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.
1963.17.1.187 PMID: 14045737
79. Giambra LM. A factor analysis of the items of the imaginal processes inventory. Journal of Clinical Psy-
chology. 1980; 36(2):383–409. https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.6120360203
80. Van Calster L, D’Argembeau A, Majerus S. Measuring individual differences in internal versus external
attention: The attentional style questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences. 2018-07;
128:25–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.014
81. Carriere JS, Seli P, Smilek D. Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wander-
ing and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne
de psychologie expe
´
rimentale. 2013; 67(1):19. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031438 PMID: 23458548
82. Wegner DM, Zanakos S. Chronic Thought Suppression. Journal of Personality. 1994; 62(4):615–640.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00311.x PMID: 7861307
83. Luciano JV, Algarabel S, Toma
´
s JM, Martı
´
nez JL. Development and validation of the thought control
ability questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences. 2005; 38(5):997–1008. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.paid.2004.06.020
84. Rassin E. The White Bear Suppression Inventory (WBSI) focuses on failing suppression attempts.
European Journal of Personality. 2003-07; 17(4):285–298. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.478
85. van Schie K, Wanmaker S, Yocarini I, Bouwmeester S. Psychometric qualities of the Thought Sup-
pression Inventory-Revised in different age groups. Personality and Individual Differences. 2016-03-
01; 91:89–97. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.060
86. Ehring T, Zetsche U, Weidacker K, Wahl K, Scho
¨
nfeld S, Ehlers A. The Perseverative Thinking Ques-
tionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. Journal
of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2011-06; 42(2):225–232. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jbtep.2010.12.003 PMID: 21315886
87. Meyer TJ, Miller ML, Metzger RL, Borkovec TD. Development and validation of the penn state worry
questionnaire. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1990; 28(6):487–495. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-
7967(90)90135-6 PMID: 2076086
88. Treynor W, Gonzalez R, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis.
Cognitive therapy and research. 2003; 27(3):247–259. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023910315561
89. Kessler R, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. The World Health Organization adult
ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychologi-
cal Medicine. 2005-02; 35(2):245–256. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291704002892 PMID:
15841682
90. Barkley RA, Murphy KR. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Clinical Workbook. 2nd ed. Guil-
ford Press; 1998.
91. Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. Journal of Statistical Software.
2012; 48(2):1–36. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02
92. Wagenmakers EJ. A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychonomic Bulletin &
Review. 2007-10-01; 14(5):779–804. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194105
93. Raftery AE. Bayesian model selection in social research. Sociological methodology. 1995; p. 111–
163. https://doi.org/10.2307/271063
94. Kline RB. Multiple-Samples Analysis and Measurement Invariance. In: Principles and Practice of
Structural Equation Modeling. 4th ed. Methodology in the Social Sciences. Guilford Press; 2016. p.
394–423.
95. Friedman NP, Miyake A. The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-var-
iable analysis. Journal of experimental psychology: General. 2004; 133(1):101. https://doi.org/10.
1037/0096-3445.133.1.101 PMID: 14979754
96. Kane MJ, Hambrick DZ, Tuholski SW, Wilhelm O, Payne TW, Engle RW. The Generality of Working
Memory Capacity: A Latent-Variable Approach to Verbal and Visuospatial Memory Span and
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 29 / 31
Reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2004-06; 133(2):189–217. https://doi.org/
10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.189 PMID: 15149250
97. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Rettinger DA, Shah P, Hegarty M. How are visuospatial working memory,
executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: General. 2001-12; 130(4):621–640. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.621 PMID:
11757872
98. Lee K, Bull R, Ho RMH. Developmental Changes in Executive Functioning. Child Development. 2013;
84(6):1933–1953. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12096 PMID: 23550969
99. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, et al. The p Factor: One
General Psychopathology Factor in the Structure of Psychiatric Disorders? Clinical Psychological Sci-
ence. 2014-03-01; 2(2):119–137. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613497473 PMID: 25360393
100. Carroll JB. Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. 1. Cambridge University
Press; 1993.
101. Little TD, Slegers DW, Card NA. A non-arbitrary method of identifying and scaling latent variables in
SEM and MACS models. Structural equation modeling. 2006; 13(1):59–72. https://doi.org/10.1207/
s15328007sem1301_3
102. Muthe
´
n LK, Muthe
´
n BO. Regressing on a residual;. Available from: https://www.statmodel.com/
download/Regressing%20on%20a%20residual.pdf.
103. Zhang B, Sun T, Cao M, Drasgow F. Using Bifactor Models to Examine the Predictive Validity of Hier-
archical Constructs: Pros, Cons, and Solutions. Organizational Research Methods. 2021-07-01; 24
(3):530–571. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428120915522
104. Berkowitz M, Stern E. Which Cognitive Abilities Make the Difference? Predicting Academic Achieve-
ments in Advanced STEM Studies. Journal of Intelligence. 2018-10-26; 6(4):48. https://doi.org/10.
3390/jintelligence6040048 PMID: 31162475
105. Smallwood J, Schooler J. The Science of Mind Wandering: Empirically Navigating the Stream of Con-
sciousness. Annual Review of Psychology. 2015; 66(1):487–518. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-
psych-010814-015331 PMID: 25293689
106. Paulhus DL. Measurement and Control of Response Bias. In: Measures of Personality and Social Psy-
chological Attitudes. Elsevier; 1991. p. 17–59. Available from: https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/
pii/B978012590241050006X.
107. Wilhelm O, Wittho
¨
ft M, Schipolowski S. Self-Reported Cognitive Failures: Competing Measurement
Models and Self-Report Correlates. Journal of Individual Differences. 2010-01; 31(1):1–14. https://doi.
org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000001
108. John OP, Srivastava S. The Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspec-
tives. In: Handbook of personality: Theory and research. New York: Guilford Press; 1999. p. 102–
138.
109. Gunthert KC, Cohen LH, Armeli S. The role of neuroticism in daily stress and coping. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology. 1999-11; 77(5):1087–1100. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.5.
1087 PMID: 10573882
110. du Pont A, Rhee SH, Corley RP, Hewitt JK, Friedman NP. Are rumination and neuroticism genetically
or environmentally distinct risk factors for psychopathology? Journal of abnormal psychology. 2019-
07; 128(5):385–396. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000430 PMID: 30985175
111. Munoz E, Sliwinski MJ, Smyth JM, Almeida DM, King HA. Intrusive thoughts mediate the association
between neuroticism and cognitive function. Personality and Individual Differences. 2013-11; 55
(8):898–903. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.07.019 PMID: 24683284
112. Segerstrom SC, Tsao JC, Alden LE, Craske MG. Worry and rumination: Repetitive thought as a con-
comitant and predictor of negative mood. Cognitive therapy and Research. 2000; 24(6):671–688.
https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005587311498
113. Carciofo R, Yang J, Song N, Du F, Zhang K. Psychometric evaluation of Chinese-language 44-item
and 10-item big five personality inventories, including correlations with chronotype, mindfulness and
mind wandering. PloS one. 2016; 11(2):e0149963. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0149963
PMID: 26918618
114. Mu¨ller M, Sindermann C, Rozgonjuk D, Montag C. Mind-wandering mediates the associations
between neuroticism and conscientiousness, and tendencies towards smartphone use disorder.
Frontiers in psychology. 2021; p. 2457. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.661541 PMID:
34526928
115. Robison MK, Miller AL, Unsworth N. A multi-faceted approach to understanding individual differences
in mind-wandering. Cognition. 2020; 198:104078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104078
PMID: 32062086
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 30 / 31
116. Vannucci M, Chiorri C. Individual differences in self-consciousness and mind wandering: Further evi-
dence for a dissociation between spontaneous and deliberate mind wandering. Personality and Indi-
vidual Differences. 2018; 121:57–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.09.022
117. Williams PG, Rau HK, Suchy Y, Thorgusen SR, Smith TW. On the validity of self-report assessment of
cognitive abilities: Attentional control scale associations with cognitive performance, emotional adjust-
ment, and personality. Psychological Assessment. 2017-05; 29(5):519–530. https://doi.org/10.1037/
pas0000361 PMID: 27504900
118. Friedman NP, Miyake A, Collins SE, DeFries JC, Corley RP, Hewitt JK. Individual differences in execu-
tive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of experimental psychology General. 2008-
05; 137(2):201–225. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201 PMID: 18473654
119. Duncan J, Williams P, Nimmo-Smith I. Learning, Intelligence, and Distraction. Attention and perfor-
mance XIV: Synergies in experimental psychology, artificial intelligence, and cognitive neuroscience.
1993; 14:323.
120. Zacks RT, Hasher L. Directed ignoring: Inhibitory regulation of working memory. In: Inhibitory pro-
cesses in attention, memory, and language. Academic Press; 1994. p. 241–264.
121. Hedge C, Powell G, Sumner P. The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reli-
able individual differences. Behavior Research Methods. 2018-06; 50(3):1166–1186. https://doi.org/
10.3758/s13428-017-0935-1 PMID: 28726177
122. Rey-Mermet A, Gade M, Oberauer K. Should we stop thinking about inhibition? Searching for individ-
ual and age differences in inhibition ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory,
and Cognition. 2018-04; 44(4):501–526. PMID: 28956944
123. Forster S. Distraction and mind-wandering under load. Frontiers in psychology. 2013; 4:283. https://
doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00283 PMID: 23734138
124. Kane MJ, Gross GM, Chun CA, Smeekens BA, Meier ME, Silvia PJ, et al. For Whom the Mind Wan-
ders, and When, Varies Across Laboratory and Daily-Life Settings. Psychological Science. 2017-09;
28(9):1271–1289. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617706086 PMID: 28719760
125. Ko
¨
nen T, Karbach J. Self-Reported Cognitive Failures in Everyday Life: A Closer Look at Their Rela-
tion to Personality and Cognitive Performance. Assessment. 2020-07-01; 27(5):982–995. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1073191118786800 PMID: 29993260
126. Snyder HR, Friedman NP, Hankin BL. Associations between task performance and self-report mea-
sures of cognitive control: Shared versus distinct abilities. Assessment. 2021; 28(4):1080–1096.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191120965694 PMID: 33084353
127. Friedman NP, Gustavson DE. Do Rating and Task Measures of Control Abilities Assess the Same
Thing? Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2022-06; 31(3):262–271. https://doi.org/10.1177/
09637214221091824 PMID: 35928929
PLOS ONE
Distractibility, ADHD, and hyperfocus
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0292215 October 25, 2023 31 / 31