RESEARCH ARTICLE
The use of social robots with children and
young people on the autism spectrum: A
systematic review and meta-analysis
Athanasia Kouroupa
ID
1,2
, Keith R. Laws
1
, Karen Irvine
1
, Silvana E. Mengoni
1
,
Alister Baird
ID
2
, Shivani Sharma
1
*
1 School of Life and Medical Sciences, University of Hertfordshire, Hatfield, United Kingdom, 2 Division of
Psychiatry, University College London, London, United Kingdom
Abstract
Background
Robot-mediated interventions show promise in supporting the development of children on
the autism spectrum.
Objectives
In this systematic review and meta-analysis, we summarize key features of available evi-
dence on robot-interventions for children and young people on the autism spectrum aged up
to 18 years old, as well as consider their efficacy for specific domains of learning.
Data sources
PubMed, Scopus, EBSCOhost, Google Scholar, Cochrane Library, ACM Digital Library,
and IEEE Xplore. Grey literature was also searched using PsycExtra, OpenGrey, British
Library EThOS, and the British Library Catalogue. Databases were searched from inception
until April (6th) 2021.
Synthesis methods
Searches undertaken across seven databases yielded 2145 articles. Forty studies met our
review inclusion criteria of which 17 were randomized control trials. The methodological
quality of studies was conducted with the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies.
A narrative synthesis summarised the findings. A meta-analysis was conducted with 12
RCTs.
Results
Most interventions used humanoid (67%) robotic platforms, were predominantly based in
clinics (37%) followed home, schools and laboratory (17% respectively) environments and
targeted at improving social and communication skills (77%). Focusing on the most common
outcomes, a random effects meta-analysis of RCTs showed that robot-mediated
PLOS ONE
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 1 / 25
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
a1111111111
OPEN ACCESS
Citation: Kouroupa A, Laws KR, Irvine K, Mengoni
SE, Baird A, Sharma S (2022) The use of social
robots with children and young people on the
autism spectrum: A systematic review and meta-
analysis. PLoS ONE 17(6): e0269800. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800
Editor: Cristina Vassalle, Fondazione Toscana
Gabriele Monasterio, ITALY
Received: March 9, 2022
Accepted: May 30, 2022
Published: June 22, 2022
Peer Review History: PLOS recognizes the
benefits of transparency in the peer review
process; therefore, we enable the publication of
all of the content of peer review and author
responses alongside final, published articles. The
editorial history of this article is available here:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800
Copyright: © 2022 Kouroupa et al. This is an open
access article distributed under the terms of the
Creative Commons Attribution License, which
permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided the original
author and source are credited.
Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are
within the paper and its Supporting Information
files.
interventions significantly improved social functioning (g = 0.35 [95%CI 0.09 to 0.61; k = 7).
By contrast, robots did not improve emotional (g = 0.63 [95%CI -1.43 to 2.69]; k = 2) or
motor outcomes (g = -0.10 [95%CI -1.08 to 0.89]; k = 3), but the numbers of trials were very
small. Meta-regression revealed that age accounted for almost one-third of the variance in
effect sizes, with greater benefits being found in younger children.
Conclusions
Overall, our findings support the use of robot-mediated interventions for autistic children and
youth, and we propose several recommendations for future research to aid learning and
enhance implementation in everyday settings.
PROSPERO registration
Our methods were preregistered in the PROSPERO database (CRD42019148981).
Introduction
With an ongoing focus on early interventions [1, 2], the functioning of children and young
people on the autism spectrum has progressively improved [3]. An array of interventions exists
to support the development of social, emotional and life skills in autistic children but with
mixed evidence of clinical effectiveness [4]. This makes it important to continue to search for
approaches that are adaptable to individual needs given the heterogeneity of the autism spec-
trum, and scalable to advance the most benefit.
Amongst the plethora of interventions, robots have emerged as a promising aid in the
development of everyday skills and as a mechanism to improve quality of life [5, 6]. Recent
studies show that robots are well-accepted by children and young people on the autism spec-
trum and are linked to positive impact on imitation skills, eye-contact, joint attention, beha-
vioural response, and repetitive and stereotyped behaviour [7, 8]. Several reviews have
summarised that individuals on the autism spectrum interact more effectively with robots
than humans to practice life-skills [7, 9, 10]. This advantage has been attributed to stimulation
through repetition, simplified facial expressions that mimic humans, and a gradual increase in
the level of challenge all acting as important scaffolds to mastering skills [912]. In other
words, robots offer a predictable and consistent interaction pattern, which is favourable to the
learning of children and young people on the autism spectrum [1315].
So-called ‘social robots’ have shown advantages in educating children and youth on the
autism spectrum in various domains, including: attention [6], learning [16], behavioural regu-
lation [17, 18] and restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviours [1921]. Social robots are
described as physically embodied agents that have some (or full) autonomy and engage in
social interactions with humans, by communicating, cooperating, and making decisions [22].
A research study classified robots based on their appearance as human-inspired, animal-
inspired, imaginary, or manmade objects and functional robots (e.g., drones) [23]. Robot plat-
forms benefit from the capacity to represent familiar social cues to children and young people
in a controlled environment (e.g., facial features such as eyes). Technological advances have
also enabled humanoid robots to represent a range of human-like functions, which is impor-
tant for children on the autism spectrum, whose perceptual processing of humans and objects
appears to be similar [24]. The emergence of social robots brings opportunity that innovative
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 2 / 25
Funding: The author(s) received no specific
funding for this work.
Competing interests: The authors have declared
that no competing interests exist.
technologies could further aid the development of skills in children and young people on the
autism spectrum through playful activities and that such interaction might positively impact
the learning process [25]. Further, rapid developments in technology mean that interventions
can be more readily personalized, a salient feature given the heterogeneity of idiosyncratic dif-
ficulties in autism [26, 27].
Several intervention programmes have explored the impact of social robots on skills devel-
opment, however evidence concerning their efficacy remains limited [28, 29]. Despite the
potential of robots in autism training, significant gaps persist in the literature. Studies have
mainly focused on reviewing the acceptability of robots to children and young people on the
autism spectrum as well as therapists delivering interventions. Research has overlooked the
variability of robot types used and their efficacy beyond the immediate intervention period [7,
22, 30]. In addition, important features such as the settings in which intervention is delivered,
and characteristics such as the number of sessions needed to bring about meaningful benefit
remains unclear. Examining if and how environment influences the efficacy of robot interven-
tions is fundamental to enhance learning gain and to consider if any setting is more suitable to
overcome the challenge of generalizing skills [30, 31]. It is important to know the outcomes
targeted by robot-mediated interventions and to consider meaningful skills development in
these domains to inform future directions for robotic research in autism, and importantly,
applied clinical value. A recent study reviewed evidence from randomised control trials
(RCTs) with children and adults on the autism spectrum, finding that most utilised humanoid
robots, focusing on outcomes such as job interview skills, gesture production and recognition,
social, mental, physical, and verbal skills [32]. While Salimi and colleagues [32] reported that
15/19 RCTs demonstrated positive gains in targeted skills development, the authors did not
undertake meta-analysis to quantify efficacy for specific clusters of skill development and this
is key if social robots are to be advanced in everyday care.
In the current systematic review and meta-analysis, we aimed to summarise the evidence-
based on the use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum, con-
sidering data from all study types and grey literature. The objectives of the review were to eval-
uate the different robot platforms that have been used with individuals on the autism
spectrum, the settings in which interventions have been implemented, the role of robots within
interventions, and the range of outcomes targeted for therapeutic gain. Within this, we
explored any specific trends related to randomised and non-randomised studies. Further, we
aimed to use meta-analysis to pool data from RCTs, widely accepted as the most rigorous
study design, to assess the efficacy of interventions for specific domains of learning.
Methods
The current systematic review and meta-analysis was completed in accordance with the Pre-
ferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) (S1 Table) [33].
The study protocol was preregistered on PROSPERO [CRD42019148981].
Identification of studies
Papers were eligible for inclusion in the review if (a) participants were diagnosed on the autism
spectrum using established diagnostic criteria (ICD-11, DSM-V or previous versions); (b) they
were aged under 18 years; (c) the study included an intervention based on any robotic plat-
form; (d) data were reported for at least one outcome against which to measure intervention
gains. We only included primary studies and all designs were considered (e.g., randomised,
case controlled, case reports). Lateral search techniques were also used to identify additional
papers for inclusion in the review.
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 3 / 25
Criteria for exclusion in the review were the following: (1) lack of separate presentation of
study outcomes for children on the autism spectrum; (2) individual aged over the age of 19; (3)
lack of recording study procedures including number, duration and frequency of robot-medi-
ated sessions; (4) no reference to the robot type/model; (5) commentary papers, protocols, sur-
veys and reviews; (6) qualitative studies; (7) qualitative elements in mixed-method designs; (8)
studies that were not published in English.
We searched the following databases: PubMed, Scopus, EBSCOhost, Google Scholar,
Cochrane Library, ACM Digital Library, and IEEE Xplore. Grey literature was also searched
using PsycExtra, OpenGrey, British Library EThOS, and the British Library Catalogue.
Search terms aligned to the following core domains: ‘autism’ AND ‘robot’ AND outcome-
specific terms aligned to the concepts of social, emotions, communication, education, aca-
demic attainment, behaviour, or health (S2 Table). Databases were searched from inception
until April (6th) 2021. Additional studies were identified through a manual search of the refer-
ences in relevant studies.
Study selection
Identified records were exported into Mendeley (v1.19.8) and duplicates removed. The first
author (AK) screened all the titles and abstracts, and a second reviewer (AB) independently
screened a random sample of 20% of the originally identified records, both using pre-deter-
mined inclusion criteria, to establish reliability for study selection. The full texts of potentially
relevant papers were retrieved and independently assessed for eligibility by the first author
(AK). Twenty percent of full text of the eligible studies were independently screened for eligi-
bility by the second reviewer (AB). There was 100% agreement between reviewers in the selec-
tion of studies which met the inclusion/exclusion criteria.
Data extraction
A pre-piloted form was used to extract data, including the following items: authors and year of
publication, the study aims, design, methods, and a summary of the outcomes. We extracted
characteristics of study participants such as mean age of all study participants, gender distribu-
tion, diagnostic tool, and intelligent quotient (IQ), where available. Sample ethnicity was also
recorded, where available. The socioeconomic status was not recorded in any of the included
studies and so these data could not be extracted.
Characteristics of the interventions were extracted across the articles and included: robot
type, duration of intervention, frequency and length of sessions, location where intervention
was delivered, and outcome measured. Other intervention characteristics such as the type of
robot used, and intervention location were also extracted. Authors of 10 separate studies were
contacted to extract information which was not clearly reported in published work (e.g., con-
firm diagnosis, session location, number and/or duration of a session, moderator of session
delivery, mean and standard deviation per participant). Three authors shared information
about their studies. Findings were summarized narratively, and where relevant, using means,
standard deviations, and percentages.
Meta-analysis
Meta-analyses were conducted using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 3.0 for Windows
[34]. We calculated Hedge’s g effect sizes (and 95% confidence intervals) for end-of-trial data
comparing robot-mediated interventions and control groups in RCTs. Hedge’s g adjusts effect
sizes according to sample size. Comparisons were made for intervention and control group at
end-of-trial on the primary outcomes of: social, emotional, and motor benefits (which
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 4 / 25
emerged as the main clusters of outcomes in RCTs). All meta-analyses used a random effects
approach. We classified effect sizes as small (0.2) medium (0.5) and large (0.8) according to
Cohen’s nomenclature. Heterogeneity was assessed using the I2 statistic, and for interpretation
we followed Cochrane guidance (Higgins et al., 2019) where I2 values were identified: 0%-40%
as might not be important; 30%-60% as may represent moderate heterogeneity; 50%-90% may
represent substantial heterogeneity; 75%-100% representing considerable heterogeneity.
Quality assessment
Two reviewers (NK and AB) independently measured the quality of the included studies using
the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies [35]. The assessment tool assesses six
components of study validity including: selection bias, study design, confounders, blinding,
data collection methods, withdrawals, and dropouts. Each component is rated as strong (1),
moderate (2), or weak (3). Each paper receives an overall mark ranging between “strong (no
weak rating)”, “moderate (one weak rating)” and “weak (two or more weak ratings)”. All stud-
ies were appraised independently by the first reviewer (NK). The second reviewer (AB)
reviewed independently 20% of the included studies. The inter-rater reliability between the
authors, using Cohen’s Kappa was ‘strong’ (0.87 agreement). Any disagreements between the
two reviewers were resolved through discussion or by consulting a third reviewer (SS). The
results of the quality analysis were further tabulated to identify any types of bias common to
the included studies (S3 and S4 Tables).
Results
The study selection process and a summary of included articles will be presented first, followed
by a general overview of the quality of research. Next, the main results will be presented
according to our review objectives.
Selection and inclusion of studies
The search generated 2145 records. After removing duplicates, 1646 records were screened,
with 151 deemed relevant and full texts reviewed, of which 44 articles reporting 40 studies
were deemed eligible for inclusion into the systematic review. The most common reason for
exclusion was the lack of information concerning diagnostic method for autism (n = 57) fol-
lowed by studies which did not meet the inclusion criteria such as reviews, protocols, surveys,
feasibility trials, opinion letters (n = 23). A smaller number of studies were excluded because of
the following reasons: 1. a new robotic platform/ intervention was developed (n = 9); 2. adults/
children with diagnoses other than autism were examined (n = 7); 3. the robot name/type was
missing (n = 4); 4. a qualitative study had been conducted (n = 2) (Fig 1).
Characteristics of included studies
The description of the study characteristics is based on 40 studies. Four articles [21, 36, 37]
had overlapping samples and were not included in the average sample size. The majority of the
studies were non-randomized (n = 23, 57%), followed by RCTs (n = 17, 43%) (Table 1).
Thirty-four studies utilised video data to analyse the study findings. The average sample size of
children and young people on the autism spectrum across the studies was 10 (range 1–30),
(M = 10.22, SD = 6.58, k = 40).
Most studies recruited more males than females, with percentage of males ranging from
67% to 100%. Ethnicity of participants was reported in nine studies (22%), of which five studies
included Chinese participants [3842], three comprised Caucasian, African American, Asian
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 5 / 25
Hispanic, Mixed African American and Caucasian, Mixed Caucasian and Hispanic and Latino
participants [20, 43, 44] and one study included Italian children [45].
Comparing RCT and non-RCT designs, the former had more complete reporting about the
autism diagnosis process (for example, from which type of healthcare professional). Only six-
teen studies (40%) measured the cognitive capacity (IQ) of children. Further characterisation
of participants (e.g., school type, parent’s demographics) was generally poor. Child/adolescent
participants ranged in age from 2 years to 16 years (M = 7.4; SD = 3.08). Overall, the studies
were published between 2008 and 2020, in Europe (k = 18, 41%) [i.e., Romania (k = 7), Portu-
gal (k = 3), France (k = 2), Italy (k = 2), Netherlands (k = 2), Belgium (k = 1) and Luxembourg
(k = 1)] followed by the United States of America (k = 11, 27%). Some studies were based in
East Asia (k = 11, 25%) including in Hong Kong (k = 7), Korea (k = 1) and Japan (k = 3). Only
one study was conducted in Canada. Most studies (k = 33, 82%) had received funding to con-
duct their work.
Quality assessment and risk of bias in included studies
The quality assessments revealed that few studies were rated as strong (k = 7), with most rates
as moderate (k = 16) or weak (k = 17) (S2 and S3 Tables). Most studies did not adequately
report on participant selection, resulting in three-quarters being rated as moderate in selection
bias (n = 30, 75%). This trend was comparable across both randomised and non-randomised
studies. The poor quality of many studies can also be partly attributed to the confounding vari-
able rating. Reporting on ethnicity, age, family or socioeconomic status of families was often
poorly described with limited matching again across most study groups (n = 29, 72%); and
again, this was as much a feature of RCTs as non-RCTs. Effort was made to contact authors for
Fig 1. PRISMA flow diagram of the study selection process.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800.g001
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 6 / 25
Table 1. Summary of study characteristics sorted by study design and mostly used robot.
Reference;
Country;
Funding
Robot group Control group IQ Robot type Adverse
events
Session
details
Risk of
bias
(overall)
Measure Outcome
RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIALS
Huskens et al.,
2013;
Netherlands;
funded [48]
N = 3 (100%
male); 8–12 years
old
Human
therapist; N = 3
(100% male);
8–12 years old
85–111 NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
5 sessions;
30 minutes;
Clinic room
Moderate Video recording No significant
between-group
differences in
self-initiated
questions at 19-
21-week
assessment.
Marino et al.,
2020
Italy; funded
[45]
N = 7 (86%
male); 4–8 years
old; Italian
Human
therapist; N = 7
(86% male); 4–8
years old;
Italian
82–121 NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
12 sessions;
90 minutes;
Laboratory
Strong Test of
Emotional
Comprehension
(TEC) &
Emotional
Lexicon Test
(ELT)
Improved
emotional
recognition and
comprehension in
robot group at
12-week
assessment.
So et al., 2018;
Hong Kong;
funded [38]
N = 7 (71%
male); 6–12 years
old; Chinese
Waitlist group
robot sessions
after research
completion;
N = 6 (83%
Males); 6–12
years old;
Chinese
49–67 NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
24 sessions;
30 minutes;
School
Weak Video recording Improved motor
imitation (e.g.,
gestural accuracy)
at 12-week
assessment for
robot group.
So et al., 2018;
Hong Kong;
funded [39]
N = 15 (87%
male); 4–6 years
old; Chinese
Waitlist group
robot sessions
after research
completion;
N = 15 (93%
Males); 4–6
years old;
Chinese
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
8 sessions;
30 minutes;
School
Weak Video recording Improved motor
imitation (e.g.,
gestural accuracy)
at 10-week
assessment for
robot group.
So et al., 2019;
Hong Kong;
funded [40]
N = 13 (85%
male); 4–6 years
old; Chinese
Waitlist group
robot sessions
after research
completion;
N = 11 (93%
male); 4–6 years
old; Chinese
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
12 sessions;
45 minutes;
Clinic room
Weak Video recording Improved
narrative skills at
12-week
assessment for
robot group.
So et al., 2019;
Hong Kong;
funded [41]
N = 12 (83%
male); 6–12 years
old; Chinese
Human
therapist;
N = 11 (91%
male); 6–12
years old;
Chinese
46–74 NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
4–5
sessions; 30
minutes;
School
Weak Video recording No significant
between-group
differences in
motor imitation
(e.g., gestural
accuracy) at
10-week
assessment.
So et al., 2020;
Hong Kong;
funded [40]
N = 12 (83%
male); 4–6 years
old; Chinese
Waitlist group
robot sessions
after research
completion
N = 11 (91%
male); 4–6 years
old; Chinese
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
9 sessions;
45 minutes;
Clinic room
Weak Video recording Improved joint
attention in robot-
based drama
sessions at 9-week
assessment.
(Continued)
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 7 / 25
Table 1. (Continued)
Reference;
Country;
Funding
Robot group Control group IQ Robot type Adverse
events
Session
details
Risk of
bias
(overall)
Measure Outcome
Srinivasan
et al., 2015;
USA; funded
[20]
N = 12 (92%
male); 5–12 years
old; Caucasian,
African
American, Asian
Hispanic, Mixed
African
American and
Caucasian,
Mixed Caucasian
and Hispanic
Human
therapist;
N = 12 (83%
male);
Comparison
group (tabletop
activities)
N = 12 (83%
male); 5–12
years old;
Caucasian,
African
American,
Asian Hispanic,
Mixed African
American and
Caucasian,
Mixed
Caucasian and
Hispanic
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot &
Rovio robot
Not
reported
32 sessions;
15 minutes;
Home
Moderate Video recording Improved gestural
imitation at
10-week
assessment in
robot group.
Srinivasan
et al., 2015;
USA; funded
[21]
(overlapping
sample)
N = 12 (92%
male); 5–12 years
old; Caucasian,
African
American, Asian
Hispanic, Mixed
African
American and
Caucasian,
Mixed Caucasian
and Hispanic
Human
therapist;
N = 12 (92%
male);
Comparison
group (tabletop
activities); 5–12
years old;
Caucasian,
African
American,
Asian Hispanic,
Mixed African
American and
Caucasian,
Mixed
Caucasian and
Hispanic
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot &
Rovio robot
Not
reported
32 sessions;
15 minutes;
Home
Weak Video recording Improved
repetitive
behaviour at
10-week
assessment for
human therapist
group.
Srinivasan
et al., 2016;
USA; funded
[37]
(overlapping
sample)
N = 12 (92%
male); 5–12 years
old; Caucasian,
African
American, Asian
Hispanic, Mixed
African
American and
Caucasian,
Mixed Caucasian
and Hispanic
Human
therapist;
N = 12 (83%
male);
Comparison
group (tabletop
activities)
N = 12 (88%
male); 5–12
years old;
Caucasian,
African
American,
Asian Hispanic,
Mixed African
American and
Caucasian,
Mixed
Caucasian and
Hispanic
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot &
Rovio robot
Not
reported
32 sessions;
45 minutes;
Home
Moderate Video recording Improved social
skills at 10-week
assessment in
robot group.
(Continued)
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 8 / 25
Table 1. (Continued)
Reference;
Country;
Funding
Robot group Control group IQ Robot type Adverse
events
Session
details
Risk of
bias
(overall)
Measure Outcome
Srinivasan
et al., 2016;
USA; funded
[37]
(overlapping
sample)
N = 12 (92%
male); 5–12 years
old; Caucasian,
African
American, Asian
Hispanic, Mixed
African
American and
Caucasian,
Mixed Caucasian
and Hispanic
Human
therapist;
N = 12 (83%
male);
Comparison
group (tabletop
activities)
N = 12 (88%
male); 5–12
years old;
Caucasian,
African
American,
Asian Hispanic,
Mixed African
American and
Caucasian,
Mixed
Caucasian and
Hispanic
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot &
Rovio robot
Not
reported
32 sessions;
45 minutes;
Home
Moderate Video recording Improved
repetitive
behaviour at
10-week
assessment for
human therapist
group.
Zheng et al.,
2020; USA;
funded [54]
N = 11 (gender
not reported);
1.64–3.14 years
old
Waitlist group
robot sessions
after research
completion
N = 9 (gender
not reported);
1.64–3.14 years
old
Mean = 58.81 NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Two
children in
waitlist and
one child in
robot group
left at first
session due
to distress
4 sessions;
10 minutes;
Clinic room
Weak Video recording No difference in
joint attention
skills at 9-week
assessment.
De Korte et al.,
2020;
Netherlands;
funded [51]
N = 24 (83%
male); 3–8 years
old
Parent Pivotal
Response
Treatment
N = 20 (85%
male); 3–8 years
old
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
20 sessions;
45 minutes;
Home
Strong Video recording Improved self-
initiation in
robot-assisted
Pivotal Response
Treatment at
3-month
assessment.
So et al., 2020;
Hong Kong;
funded [47]
N = 18 –Tier 1
(N = 6 (67%
male), Tier 2
N = 6 (100%
male), Tier 3
(N = 6; 100%
male); Tier 1
received the
intervention
earlier than Tiers
2 and 3); all aged
6–8 years old;
Chinese
Not applicable <70 HUMANE–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
6 sessions;
30 minutes;
School
Moderate Video recording Improved joint
attention at 4–8
weeks assessment
in all Tiers.
Yun et al.,
2017; Korea;
funded [67]
N = 8(100%
male); 4–7 years
old
Human
therapist; N = 7
(100% male);
4–7 years old
>60 iRobiQ &
CARO–
Humanoid
robot
None 8 sessions;
30–40
minutes;
Unknown
location
Strong Video recording No significant
between-group
differences in
eye-contact at
10-week
assessment.
(Continued)
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 9 / 25
Table 1. (Continued)
Reference;
Country;
Funding
Robot group Control group IQ Robot type Adverse
events
Session
details
Risk of
bias
(overall)
Measure Outcome
Costescu et al.,
2015; Romania;
funded [16]
N = 12 (74%
male); 6–12 years
old
Human
therapist;
N = 15 (74%
male); 6–12
years old
Not reported Keepon-
humanoid
snowman
robot
Not
reported
6 sessions;
120
minutes;
School
Moderate Video recording Improved
emotional
intensity and
reduced frequency
of irrational
beliefs in robot
group.
Pop et al.,
2013; Romania;
funded [55]
N = 7 (100%
male); 4–9 years
old
Computer-
based session;
N = 6/ control
group no
intervention;
N = 7; (100%
male); 4–9 years
old
Not reported Probo–
Mammoth
robot
Not
reported
1 session;
10–15
minutes;
Clinic room
Strong Video recording
and 7-point
Likert scale
Decreased level of
prompt in robot
group.
Pop et al.,
2014; Romania;
funded [57]
N = 5 (100%
male); 4–7 years
old
Human
therapist; N = 6
(100% male);
4–7 years old
>70 Probo–
Mammoth
robot
Not
reported
1 session;
unknown
duration;
Clinic room
Strong Video recording
and 7-point
Likert scale
Improved level of
engagement in
robot group.
Simut et al.,
2016; Belgium;
funded [59]
N = 30 (90%
male); 5–8 years
old
Human
therapist;
N = 30 (90%
male); 5–8 years
old
70–119 Probo–
Mammoth
robot
Not
reported
1 session,
15 minutes;
School
Moderate Video recording No significant
between-group
differences in
social skills (e.g.,
eye-contact, joint
attention).
Kim et al.,
2013; USA;
funded [43]
N = 24 (87%
male); 4–12 years
old; white, two
were black and
two were
Hispanic or
Latino
Human
therapist;
N = 24 (87%
male); 4–12
years old; white,
two were black
and two were
Hispanic or
Latino
72–119 Pleo–
Dinosaur
robot
Not
reported
1 session; 6
minutes;
Clinic room
Moderate Video recording No significant
between-group
differences in
number of
utterances.
Kim et al.,
2015; USA;
funded [36]
(overlapping
sample)
N = 24 (87%
male); 4–12 years
old
Human
therapist;
N = 24 (87%
male); 4–12
years old
72–119 Pleo–
Dinosaur
robot
Not
reported
1 session; 6
minutes;
Clinic room
Moderate Video recording Improved level of
enjoyment and
number of words
in robot group.
NON-RANDOMISED CONTROLLED TRIALS
Huskens et al.,
2015; USA;
funded [49]
N = 3 pairs of
1ASD:1sibling
(67% male); 5–10
years old
Not applicable >80 NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Aggression
to sibling
6–8
sessions; 30
minutes;
Clinic room
Moderate Video recording No significant
difference in
collaborative
behaviour at
12-week
assessment.
Kaboski et al.,
2015; USA;
funded [50]
N = 8 pairs of
1ASD:1TD
(100% male); 12–
17 years old
Not applicable Mean
ASD = 106
Mean
TD = 112
NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
5 sessions;
180
minutes;
Robotic
camp
Strong Social Anxiety
Scale for
Children-Revised
(SASCR), Social
Anxiety Scale
Adolescents
(SAS-A), Social
Skills
Improvement
System (SSIS)
Partial success.
Significant
decrease in social
anxiety for ASD
group only. No
significant
changes in social
skills for both
groups at 2-week
assessment.
(Continued)
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 10 / 25
Table 1. (Continued)
Reference;
Country;
Funding
Robot group Control group IQ Robot type Adverse
events
Session
details
Risk of
bias
(overall)
Measure Outcome
So et al., 2016;
Hong Kong;
not reported
[46]
N = 20 (75%
male); 6–12 years
old; Chinese;
Not applicable 51–72 NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
8 sessions;
30 minutes;
School
Weak Unclear Improved motor
imitation (e.g.,
gestural accuracy)
at 12–14 week
assessment for
robot group.
Tapus et al.,
2012; Romania;
not reported
[52]
N = 4 (100%
Male); 2–6 years
old
Human
therapist; N = 4
(100% Male);
2–6 years old
Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
23–26
sessions;
2–5
minutes
with
10minutes
break;
unclear
duration;
Clinic room
Moderate Video recording Partial success.
Mixed results for
eye-contact,
initiations,
attention between
groups at 4-week
assessment.
Individual data
presented per
child.
Warren et al.,
2015; USA;
funded [53]
N = 6 (100%
male); 2.5–4
years old
Not applicable Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
4 sessions;
unclear
duration;
Laboratory
Weak Video recording Improved
attention at
2-week
assessment.
Zheng et al.,
2016; USA; not
reported [44]
N = 6 (100%
male); 2.5–4
years old;
Caucasian
Not applicable Not reported NAO–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
6 sessions;
unclear
duration;
Laboratory
Weak Video recording The robot
attracted the
attention at
8-month
assessment.
Kumazaki
et al., 2018;
Japan; funded
[68]
N = 11 (82%
male); mean
age = 15.91
Human
therapist;
N = 11 (82%
male) mean
age = 15.91
Not reported ACTROID-F
& CommU–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
1 session; 5
minutes;
Clinic room
Moderate Audio recording Improved in
length self-
disclosure
statements in
CommU (simple)
robot group.
Kumazaki
et al., 2018b;
Japan; funded
[82]
N = 16 (75%
male); 5–6 years
old
Human
therapist;
N = 12 (58%
male); 5–6 years
old
>70 CommU–
Humanoid
robot
one child in
robot group
distressed–
unable to
complete
session
1 session;
15 minutes;
Unknown
location
Moderate Video recording Improved joint
attention in robot
group.
Yoshikawa
et al., 2019;
Japan; funded
[70]
N = 4 (100%
male); 15–18
years old
Human
therapist; N = 4
(100% male);
15–18 years old
Not reported Actroid-F–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
5 sessions;
one day;
Laboratory
Weak Video recording
& eye tracker
Improved eye-
contact in robot
group.
Srinivasan
et al., 2013;
USA; not
reported [71]
N = 1 (100%
male); 7 years old
Child-led
condition;
N = 1 (100%
male); 7 years
old
Not reported Isobot–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
8 sessions;
30 minutes;
Unknown
location
Moderate Video recording;
Sensory
Integration and
Praxis Test
(SIPT)
Improved motor
imitation skills in
robot group at
6-week
assessment.
Srinivasan &
Bhat, 2014;
USA; funded
[66]
N = 2 (100%
male); 7–8 years
old
Not applicable Not reported Isobot–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
8 sessions;
30 minutes;
Home
Moderate Video recording Decreasing
attention at
6-week
assessment.
Costa et al.,
2018;
Luxembourg;
funded [19]
N = 15 (100%
male); 4–14 years
old
Human
therapist;
N = 15 (100%
male); 4–14
years old
80–120 Qtrobot–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
1 session;
1.5–4
minutes;
Human vs
robot;
Laboratory
Moderate Video recording Improved
attention and
repetitive
behaviours in
robot group.
(Continued)
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 11 / 25
Table 1. (Continued)
Reference;
Country;
Funding
Robot group Control group IQ Robot type Adverse
events
Session
details
Risk of
bias
(overall)
Measure Outcome
Duquette et al.,
2008; Canada;
funded [14]
N = 2 (100%
male); 4–5 years
old
Human
therapist; N = 2
(50% male); 5
years old
Not reported Tito–
humanoid
robot
Not
reported
22 sessions;
3–4
minutes;
Laboratory
Weak Video recording Partial success.
Mixed findings in
imitation (e.g.,
verbal, motor,
facial) skills
between groups at
7-week
assessment.
Scassellati
et al., 2018;
USA; funded
[69]
N = 12 (58%
male); 6–12 years
old
Not applicable >70 No name–
Humanoid
robot
Not
reported
30 sessions;
30 minutes;
Home
Weak Video and audio
recor6ding
Improved social
skills (e.g.,
initiations, joint
attention eye-
contact,
engagement) at
4-week
assessment.
Pop et al.,
2013; Romania;
funded [56]
N = 3 (100%
male); 5–6 years
old
Not applicable Not reported Probo—
Mammoth
robot
Not
reported
1 session;
Clinic room
Strong Video recording
and qualitative
notes
Improved
emotional
recognition.
Simut et al.,
2012; Romania;
funded [58]
N = 4 (50%
male); 4–9 years
old
Human
therapist; N = 4
(50% male); 4–9
years old
Not reported Probo—
Mammoth
robot
Not
reported
6 sessions;
15 minutes;
Clinic room
Moderate 7-point Likert
scale
Decreased level of
prompt in robot
group.
Vanderborght
et al., 2012;
Romania;
funded [60]
N = 4 (50%
male); 4–9 years
old
Human
therapist; N = 4
(50% male); 4–9
years old
Not reported Probo—
Mammoth
robot
Not
reported
6–8
sessions;
10–20
minutes;
Clinic room
Moderate Video recording Decreased level of
prompt in robot
group at 4-week
assessment.
Silva et al.,
2018
Portugal; not
reported [61]
N = 10 (100%
male); 6–9 years
old
Living dog;
N = 10 (100%
male); 6–9 years
old
Not reported Zoomer–Dog
robot
Not
reported
3 sessions;
10 minutes;
Home
Weak Video recording Improved
emotional
regulation in
living dog
condition at
4-week
assessment.
Silva et al.,
2019
Portugal;
funded [18]
N = 10 (100%
male); 6–9 years
old
Living dog;
N = 10 (100%
male); 6–9 years
old
Not reported Zoomer–Dog
robot
Not
reported
1 session; 3
minutes;
Home
Weak Video recording Improved
emotional
regulation and
social
communication in
living dog
condition at
4-week
assessment.
Silva et al.,
2020; Portugal;
funded [62]
N = 10 (100%
male); 5–8 years
old
Living dog
N = 10 (100%
male); 5–8 years
old
Not reported Zoomer–Dog
robot
Not
reported
1 session;
not
reported
minutes;
Home
Moderate Video recording Improved
imitation in living
dog condition.
Puyon &
Giannopulu,
2013
France; not
reported [62]
Game group;
N = 11 (72%
male); 7–8 years
old
No game group;
N = 11 (72%
male); 7–8 years
old
Not reported "POL"–
chicken robot
Not
reported
1 session;
10 minutes;
Clinic room
Weak Video recording Improved eye-
contact, number
of words, better
posture in game
play robot
condition.
(Continued)
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 12 / 25
further information, and five author responses were received, which resulted in information
being classified as missing and thus reducing study quality. Finally, outcome assessors (e.g.,
researchers) were not blinded in (n = 38, 95%) of the studies, including often in RCTs as well
as non-RCTs. Promisingly, most studies (n = 36, 90%) used appropriate methods to collect
data including video data. Studies provided details about the position of the cameras and the
use of at least two coders including inter-rater reliability between coders. Most studies (n = 28,
70%) also reported the number of participants approached, screened, and completed the
intervention.
Robot types
Four robot types were used that can be characterised according to their appearance in the fol-
lowing categories: humanoid, animaloid, and other. A humanoid robot is distinguished by its
resemblance to the human body. In general, a humanoid robot has a head, torso, two arms
and legs. Some humanoid robots may have facial characteristics including eyes, nose and
mouth whereas other humanoids may model part of the body from the waist up. Humanoid
robots were used in 67% (27 out of 40) of the included studies. The robot platforms that facili-
tated a session with children and young people on the autism spectrum were the following:
NAO, QTrobot, CommU, ACTROID-F, Isobot, Tito, iRobiQ, Caro, Keepon and HUMANE.
The most frequently used robot was NAO which was used in 17 studies [20, 3842, 4454].
Studies used humanoid robots to examine a range of skills including eye-contact, imitation,
joint attention, social skills and emotional regulation.
The use of animaloid (or animal-like) robots, such as an elephant, chicken, dinosaur and
dogs, was examined in 11 out of 40 studies (27%). In the review, the Probo robot (elephant-
like) was referenced in six studies [5560]. Other animaloid robots were the robot, Pleo, [36],
the dog robot Zoomer, [17, 18, 61] and POL (chicken-like) [62]. These robots facilitated ses-
sions focusing on eye contact, imitation, joint attention and social skills.
The remaining ‘other’ robot category included a robotic arm and a plant robot. The robotic
arm was used with children and young people on the autism spectrum to examine imitation
and eye-contact [63]. The plant robot, called ‘Pekoppa’, was fully programmable with inte-
grated sensors that allowed the robot to model a range of functions. Pekoppa was used with
neurotypical children and young people and children and young people on the autism spec-
trum to compare the differences in heart rate, verbal fluency, and emotional response [64].
When exploring trends across study type, it appeared that humanoid robots were used in
both RCTs (n = 12) and non-RCTs (n = 14). In particular, the robot NAO was utilised in 10
Table 1. (Continued)
Reference;
Country;
Funding
Robot group Control group IQ Robot type Adverse
events
Session
details
Risk of
bias
(overall)
Measure Outcome
Pierno et al.,
2008
Italy; funded
[63]
N = 12 (50%
male); aged 10–
13 years old
Human
therapist;
N = 12 (50%
male); aged 10–
13 years old
Not reported Robotic arm–
industrial
robot
Not
reported
1 session;
60 minutes;
Laboratory
Weak Video recording Improved
attention in robot
group.
Giannopulu
et al., 2014
France; not
reported [64]
N = 15 (73%
male); 6–7 years
old
Human
therapist;
N = 15 (73%
male); 6–7 years
old
Not reported “Pekoppa”–
other robot
Not
reported
1 session;
15 minutes;
Clinic room
Weak Unclear Improved
expressie
language in robot
group.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800.t001
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 13 / 25
RCTs compared to six non-RCTs (Table 1). Non-RCTs were therefore more likely to include a
broader range of robot platforms.
Settings
Intervention sessions with robots took place in five different settings. The most common loca-
tion was autism centres/clinic rooms (n = 15) followed by home (n = 7), school (n = 7) and lab-
oratories (n = 7). RCTs showed a trend to be more likely to take place in autism centres/clinics
(n = 7 out of 17; 41% versus n = 8 or in a familiar environment such as school (n = 6 out of 17;
35% versus n = 1 out of 23; 4%). Sessions at home were more common in non-RCTs (n = 5;
22%) compared to RCTs (n = 2; 12%). Similarly, sessions in a laboratory were more common
in non-RCTs (n = 6; 26%) than RCTs (n = 1; 5%) (S5 Table).
Sixty-five percent (n = 11) of RCTs and 78% (n = 18) of non-RCTs indicated a positive ben-
efit of intervention. The mean duration of robot-intervention was 8.45 sessions and each ses-
sion lasting an average of 33.5 minutes—most likely occurring once (22%) or twice weekly
(28%) over the intervention period, though there was considerable variability (Table 2). The
first session was usually a familiarisation meeting with the child and the robot/play partner.
Intervention sessions tended to be longer in RCTs, with a mean of 35 (range: 6–120) minutes
versus 27 (range: 3–180) minutes in non-RCTs. Similarly, the number of sessions was greater
in RCTs with a mean of 9 sessions (range: 1–32) compared to 7 (range: 1–30) in non-RCTs.
Notably, play partners across studies were more often ‘professionals’ for example, researchers
or healthcare workforce (90%). Children and young people on the autism spectrum had indi-
vidual sessions apart from in three studies [49, 50, 65].
Robot’s role in intervention
During intervention, robots took on the role of a social interface. Hence the robot moved its
head and eyes to express emotions via facial expressions (e.g., happy, sad) or verbally, became
a storyteller, an imitation agent, an intermediate to attract the eye gaze of the child on the
autism spectrum or facilitated collaboration within a small group of two children and young
people or an object where the child on the autism spectrum engaged in free play. In most
Table 2. Summary of features of robot-mediated intervention.
Robot session characteristics
Number of sessions Mean (SD; range) 8.45 (9.52; 1–32 sessions)
Duration per session (mins) Mean (SD; range) 32 (35.85; 3–180 mins)
Session frequency N (%)
Single session 2 (5%)
Daily 4 (10%)
Once a week 9 (22%)
Twice a week 11 (28%)
Three times a week 1 (2%)
Varied frequency 4 (10%)
Not reported 9 (23%)
Play partner N (%)
Researcher 26 (65%)
Child/Clinical Psychologist/ Psychotherapist 10 (25%)
Parent 1 (2%)
No play partner 3 (8%)
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800.t002
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 14 / 25
studies (n = 38, 95%), children and young people engaged in a triadic relationship with an
adult/therapist where the robot acted as a mediator. A typical session involved the play partner
controlling the robot via a laptop/computer. Two studies used a fully autonomous robot to
play independently without the guidance of an adult partner. The control group in RCTs was
often a human therapist engaging the child with the same or similar activities apart from five
studies that used a waitlist and so, received the robot session after study completion. It was
unclear whether the children in waitlist studies were receiving no treatment interventions as
part of their educational and community settings.
Targeted skills and outcomes
Studies included in this review targeted a number of skills that can be clustered into 3 main
categories: (1) social and communication skills [narrative skills (n = 1), self-initiated questions
(n = 4), engagement (n = 2), self-disclosure (n = 1), collaborative play (n = 1), level of prompt-
ing (n = 3), joint attention (n = 6), eye-contact (n = 6), imitation (n = 7)]; (2) emotional devel-
opment [recognition and/or understanding (n = 2), emotional regulation (n = 4)]; and (3)
motor skills [stereotyped or repetitive behaviour (n = 1)]. Over two-thirds (n = 29; 72%) of the
articles reported a positive impact of a robot-mediated intervention in children and young
people on the autism spectrum. Less than a quarter of the included articles (n = 10, 25%)
reported no difference in targeted skills development. Finally, one article reported a decline in
attention skills during the intervention period [66].
The majority of the targeted skills were measured through the examination of video record-
ings and coding procedures completed by researchers whereas only two studies [45, 50] used
standardised assessment tools to examine social skills and emotional comprehension
(Table 1). Nine studies [20, 3842, 45, 55, 67] used blinded researchers to administer the ques-
tionnaires and one study [50] relied on parent-reported outcomes. Another three studies [55,
57, 58] utilised child self-report methods, three used qualitative methods (e.g., audio recording,
notes by researchers) [56, 68, 69] and two used eye-tracking [70] or a sensory integration and
praxis test [71] in conjunction with video recordings. Two studies made explicit reference to
the benefits of the robot-mediated intervention at two weeks [48] and four weeks [40] follow-
ing the end of the intervention. Finally, Marino and colleagues [45] reported that children on
the autism spectrum in both groups (robot and human) spontaneously practiced the trained
skills addressing generalisation issues. No studies included evaluation of health economics
related to intervention delivery.
Meta-analysis
Hedge’s g was calculated for RCTs examining outcomes relating to: social (k = 7), emotional
(k = 2) and motor (k = 3) abilities and for all three areas combined (k = 12). This provided a
total of 346 participants (175 assigned to robot and 171 assigned to control conditions). The
control condition of the included studies comprised of children in a human therapist group
apart from three studies [39, 41, 42] that had a waitlist group (where the children received the
robot session after study completion).
Nine RCTs were excluded from the analyses because of: (1) overlapping samples [20, 21, 36,
37]; and (2) use of waitlist group and/or no reporting (or sharing when contacted directly) of
means, SDs or effect sizes [38, 42, 54, 55, 57]. The included RCTs had quite good quality rat-
ings: strong (k = 4), moderate (k = 6) and weak (k = 3). All three of the weak ratings were for
the studies by So and colleagues [3941] and all were assessing motor outcomes.
RCTs providing sufficient data for emotion-based outcomes to be examined revealed a
nonsignificant effect size (g = 0.63 [95%CI -1.43 to 2.69]; k = 2). Heterogeneity was high
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 15 / 25
(I2 = 88.65). For trials assessing motor outcomes, the effect size was again non-significant (g =
-0.10 [95%CI -1.08 to 0.89]; k = 3) and heterogeneity was again high (I2 = 79.63). For social
outcomes, the effect size was significant (g = 0.35 [95%CI 0.09 to 0.61; k = 7) and heterogeneity
was low (I2 = 0.00). When we combined all three sets of outcomes to assess any pooled benefit
of robot-mediated interventions (Fig 2), the effect size was significant (g = 0.33 [95%CI 0.08 to
0.57; k = 12) and heterogeneity was moderate and significant (I2 = 54.48). Visual analysis of
funnel plots did not suggest any asymmetry and evidence of obvious publication bias (S1 Fig).
Although there is no definitive minimum number of studies required for meta-regression,
we follow the general recommendation of at least 6 to 10 studies for a continuous variable
(Higgins et al., 2019). Given this, we used meta-regression to assess possible moderators across
all 12 RCTs in the meta-analysis. We found that age was a significant moderator (z = -1.97,
df = 12, p = .02) (S2 Fig), with effect sizes being significantly larger in younger samples. Indeed,
age accounted for nearly a third of the variance in effect sizes (analog r
2
= .32). None of the
other continuous variable moderators were significant including: total length of time in ses-
sions (z = 0.40 df = 12, p = .35); proportion of male participants (z = 0.97, df = 12, p = .17); and
IQ (z = 1.44 df = 8 p = .07).
We also used sub-group analysis to see if the context (home, school, clinic) impacted effect
sizes across all RCTs. This analysis showed a significant impact of robots in the clinic (g = 0.57
(95%0.16 to 0.98; k = 5) with low heterogeneity (I2 = 21.96). By contrast robots were not effica-
cious in either the home g = 0.16 (-0.56 to 0.89; k = 2; I2 = 55.55) or in school g = -0.16 (-0.85
to 0.53; k = 4, I2 = 75.19), though few trials examined the latter.
Discussion
The current systematic review summarises evidence on the use of social robots with children
and young people on the autism spectrum. We aimed to examine the typology of robots, the
Fig 2. Forest plot showing efficacy of robot intervention on emotional, motor and social outcome variables.
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800.g002
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 16 / 25
settings of robot-mediated interventions, the function of the robot during intervention and the
specific skills targeted for therapeutic gain. Notably, the current review provides the first meta-
analysis to estimate the efficacy of robots to bring about meaningful gains, particularly in social
processing. We also highlight key moderators of effect sizes–these include age, with younger
individuals appearing to benefit more and with effects being significantly larger in the clinic
compared to school. Additionally, we found that effect sizes were not moderated by the length
of therapeutic intervention. This suggests that relatively brief forms of robot-based interven-
tions might be helpful (most protocols include an average of 8 sessions each lasting approxi-
mately 30 minutes), with meaningful improvement in social communication at least in the
immediate post-intervention period. Longer-term follow-up was lacking and limits the extent
to which we can generalize learned skills to daily life. For example, of the RCTs included in the
review, the median duration of follow-up data reported was 10 weeks since baseline assess-
ment. To understand the generalization of skills, and both to support commissioning decisions
and to set parent expectations about intervention outcomes, longer-term data will be essential.
Most studies in this review focused on evaluating humanoid robotic platforms, with a small
minority assessing animaloid types. In line with the social nature of the robots, the most com-
mon outcomes could be grouped into three clusters, with most studies focusing on social com-
munication, emotional outcomes, and motor imitation. Most often outcomes were assessed
using video data over other assessment types. However, in studies using video-recording data,
raters were not blinded in 95%, typically because the data being coded was during the interven-
tion when the robot is clearly visible. To overcome this bias, we advance that future trials use
naturalistic observations (e.g., free play) to better assess the extent of skills generalisation and
by raters who are blinded to the intervention the child/young person received.
The most common intervention settings were autism centres/clinics, with fewer studies
based in other everyday learning environments such as the child’s school and home. In this
context, a recent qualitative study found that educators are not ‘uncritically approving’ of the
use of social robots in schools [72]. Though robots are recognised as being motivating, engag-
ing, predicable, and consistent, educators have called for clear protocols on how and why
robots are being used to ensure effective learning support [72]. Few school-based RCTs could
be included in our meta-analysis and so, the non-significant effect is unsurprising given the
heterogeneity and lack of power. Future research needs to optimise intervention protocols and
their practicability in educational settings, as well as effective training and on-going support
for the educators involved. Though autism centres/clinics currently yield the clearest therapeu-
tic benefit, everyday settings potentially offer more feasible routes to embed intervention and
may be more cost-effective, though health economic evaluation data are currently lacking. Fur-
ther, community settings have been identified as underutilised in autism research and offer a
naturalistic environment where a range of expertise can be harnessed for the shared goal of
improving everyday functioning for both children and families [73]. Effectively integrating
robots in a range of settings remains crucial to translate research into practice.
In 80% of the studies, the robots acted to mediate between the child and play partner, who
typically controlled the robot through a keypad. Although some robots were designed with the
architecture of being autonomous (e.g., NAO), human therapists use robots as focal points to
engage children and youth on the autism spectrum in a session in case they find human inter-
action challenging [15, 74]. Human therapists also use robots to demonstrate movements and/
or facial expressions so that children can mimic these. In this way, robots preserve a human
therapist’s time and energy [10, 74, 75] whilst also benefitting from consistency as robots are
more likely to produce the same movement or expression each time. Unsurprisingly our find-
ings evidence the advantage of social robot-mediated interventions in learning or skill
domains that map onto social communication. The meta-analytic evidence here provides
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 17 / 25
support for a small-moderate benefit of robots on social-related outcomes; however, specific
impact on emotional and motor outcomes remains elusive because of the small numbers of tri-
als, the small samples per trial and the high heterogeneity currently associated with those out-
comes–all of which reduce power to detect efficacy in the emotion and motor domains.
Turning to risk of bias assessment, most studies were rated as either weak or moderate
using the Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies [35]. Only seven (17%) studies
were rated as strong, and five of these were RCTs, where higher standards of rigour and report-
ing might be expected. Common weaknesses related to selection bias [k = 36 (90%); 15 RCTs;
21 non-RCTs] and the reporting on confounding variables [k = 35 (87%); 17 RCTs; 20 non-
RCTs]. Other researchers have also commented on selection bias being common in autism
research, particularly the exclusion of youth with a diagnosis of autism and intellectual disabil-
ity [76]. Similarly, many studies did not assess intelligence. The current meta-analysis found a
trend toward larger effect sizes in samples with higher IQ; however, the meta-analysis had data
missing from four RCTs and the samples were somewhat bimodal with two studies having a
mean IQs in the 5–60 range, while the remainder were 90 to 105. Transparency and consis-
tency in reporting sample characteristics is therefore essential in future research. This will help
delineate for whom robot-mediated interventions are more effective, therefore allowing better
targeting and adaptation of intervention protocols.
Finally, the increase in research studies in the field of autism and robot-mediated interven-
tion in the past 15 years and the proportion of studies that have received funding clearly evi-
dences the growth in interest in this form of therapy for autistic individuals, mostly in the
United States and Europe. The relative lack of research on autism and social robotics from
other countries may however signal a need for more global perspectives on human-robot
interaction and cultural influences on autism intervention design [77]. For example, Hashim
& Yussof [77] suggest that robots could be humanised more to support ethical, spiritual and
religious learning, acting to increase cross-cultural appeal in autism research. Given the out-
comes of our meta-analyses, we encourage approaches that seek to adapt interventions for
cross-cultural benefit.
Strengths and limitations
As far as we are aware, the current review is both the first to be preregistered and the first to
meta-analyse some evidence on robot-mediated intervention for autistic children and young
people. In doing so, we have provided novel insights absent from other reviews [7, 32]. Our
search of the grey literature generated articles that are included in this review and so, offers a
more balanced picture of the current literature, minimising potential for publication bias. Evi-
dence from the meta-analysis did not point to any obvious publication bias amongst RCTs.
Nonetheless, the number of RCTs that provided sufficient data for meta-analysis were small
and the findings should be interpreted in that context. Nonetheless, we believe this new evi-
dence will be helpful to researchers, trialists, clinicians, educators, and parents, especially as
the field of technology-assisted learning in autism is seeing an expansion [7881]. Other limi-
tations might be the large number of articles (k = 102) we excluded owing to their poor report-
ing on autism diagnosis. Systematically excluding these studies was in line with our
preregistered inclusion criteria but may have excluded data from the true population of inter-
est. Studies might however benefit from using clear diagnostic criteria. Further, the evidence
from meta-analysis should be considered with the caveat that we could derive data from only
12 RCTs, although we did manage to capture data from almost three quarters of the trials (12/
17). We also note that our examination of moderator variables was limited to analyses that
were pooled across all three outcomes (social, emotional and motor) in order to obtain
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 18 / 25
sufficient data points. Despite such limitations, the findings for social outcomes look especially
promising, while those relating to emotional and motor abilities require further studies. We
were unable to report on adverse events as these data have been poorly reported in studies.
Only four studies reported adverse events [49, 54, 67, 82]. Finally, as commented on earlier,
the studies included in this review have reported on the outcome of brief exposure and its
impact at best in the immediate post intervention or short follow-up period. As such, though
robot mediated interventions appear to be promising, the extent to which skills are generalised
into everyday life and sustained is unclear.
Future recommendations
The outcomes of the review suggest the emergence of considerable interest in evaluating the
therapeutic benefits of social robots for children and young people on the autism spectrum.
Robot (humanoid and animaloid) platforms are suggested as suitable to personalise, scalable
and economical and so offer immense opportunity as a form of autism intervention [83, 84].
For intervention benefits to be maximised, however, better reporting across study designs on
sample recruitment and characteristics and adverse events, as well as further standardisation
of outcome measures is needed. Further, clinical utility will remain limited without evaluation
from randomised designs that assess the evidence of immediate as well as more sustained treat-
ment gains. It has been emphasised that any intervention should be implemented consistently
across settings and multiple professionals working together to support children and young
people on the autism spectrum will increase the likelihood of more rapid progress [85]. Design
and evaluation of robotic research would benefit from a multi-disciplinary approach to har-
ness technological developments, methodological considerations, and evaluation of beha-
vioural and psychological outcomes. Further, building consensus across the social robotics
research community about intervention evaluation would be advantageous and can draw on
existing approaches that have informed similar frameworks in other areas of autism interven-
tion [86].
Conclusions
Humanoid robots are the most common form of robot intervention employed with children
and young people on the autism spectrum. Intervention protocols tend to be brief, and usually
implemented in autism centres/clinics, home or to a lesser extent, at school, where robots typi-
cally take on the role of a therapeutic mediator. Evidence from the current meta-analyses sug-
gests that effects are larger when trials have been conducted in the clinic rather than at home
or in schools, and for younger children suggesting better developmental match. Current
research findings however should be interpreted cautiously given the lack of high-quality RCT
evidence. To increase assessment of clinical effectiveness, this review identifies a need for more
research based on experimental designs and with transparent reporting on sample selection,
characteristics, and adverse events, as well as assessment of intervention gains beyond the
immediate study period.
Supporting information
S1 Fig. Funnel plot exploring publication bias.
(TIF)
S2 Fig. The impact of age on robot interventions.
(TIF)
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 19 / 25
S1 Table. PRISMA 2020 checklist.
(DOCX)
S2 Table. Search terms per bibliographic database.
(DOCX)
S3 Table. Quality assessment for included studies.
(DOCX)
S4 Table. Individual study quality assessment overview.
(DOCX)
S5 Table. Vote count outcomes by setting.
(DOCX)
Author Contributions
Conceptualization: Athanasia Kouroupa, Karen Irvine, Silvana E. Mengoni, Shivani Sharma.
Data curation: Athanasia Kouroupa.
Formal analysis: Athanasia Kouroupa, Keith R. Laws.
Methodology: Athanasia Kouroupa, Keith R. Laws, Alister Baird.
Project administration: Athanasia Kouroupa.
Software: Keith R. Laws.
Supervision: Karen Irvine, Silvana E. Mengoni, Shivani Sharma.
Validation: Athanasia Kouroupa, Keith R. Laws.
Visualization: Athanasia Kouroupa, Keith R. Laws.
Writing – original draft: Athanasia Kouroupa.
Writing – review & editing: Athanasia Kouroupa, Keith R. Laws, Karen Irvine, Silvana E.
Mengoni, Alister Baird, Shivani Sharma.
References
1. Landa RJ. Efficacy of early interventions for infants and young children with, and at risk for, autism spec-
trum disorders. International Review of Psychiatry. 2018 Jan 2; 30(1):25–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/
09540261.2018.1432574 PMID: 29537331
2. Fuller EA, Kaiser AP. The effects of early intervention on social communication outcomes for children
with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2020
May; 50(5):1683–700. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-03927-z PMID: 30805766
3. Volkmar FR, Jackson SL, Hart L. Transition issues and challenges for youth with autism spectrum disor-
ders. Pediatric Annals. 2017 Jun 1; 46(6):e219–23. https://doi.org/10.3928/19382359-20170519-03
PMID: 28599026
4. Rodgers M, Marshall D, Simmonds M, Le Couteur A, Biswas M, Wright K, et al. Interventions based on
early intensive applied behaviour analysis for autistic children: a systematic review and cost-effective-
ness analysis. Health Technology Assessment (Winchester, England). 2020 Jul; 24(35):1. https://doi.
org/10.3310/hta24350 PMID: 32686642
5. Valadão CT, Goulart C, Rivera H, Caldeira E, Bastos Filho TF, Frizera-Neto A, et al. Analysis of the use
of a robot to improve social skills in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research on Biomedical
Engineering. 2016 Apr; 32:161–75.
6. Scassellati B, Admoni H, Matarić M. Robots for use in autism research. Annual review of biomedical
engineering. 2012 Aug 15; 14:275–94. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-bioeng-071811-150036 PMID:
22577778
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 20 / 25
7. Pennisi P, Tonacci A, Tartarisco G, Billeci L, Ruta L, Gangemi S, et al. Autism and social robotics: A
systematic review. Autism Research. 2016 Feb; 9(2):165–83. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1527 PMID:
26483270
8. van den Berk-Smeekens I, van Dongen-Boomsma M, De Korte MW, Den Boer JC, Oosterling IJ,
Peters-Scheffer NC, et al. Adherence and acceptability of a robot-assisted Pivotal Response Treatment
protocol for children with autism spectrum disorder. Scientific reports. 2020 May 15; 10(1):1–1.
9. Begum M, Serna RW, Yanco HA. Are robots ready to deliver autism interventions? A comprehensive
review. International Journal of Social Robotics. 2016 Apr; 8(2):157–81.
10. Diehl JJ, Schmitt LM, Villano M, Crowell CR. The clinical use of robots for individuals with autism spec-
trum disorders: A critical review. Research in autism spectrum disorders. 2012 Jan 1; 6(1):249–62.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2011.05.006 PMID: 22125579
11. Fong T, Nourbakhsh I, Dautenhahn K. A survey of socially interactive robots. Robotics and autonomous
systems. 2003 Mar 31; 42(3–4):143–66.
12. Willemse C, Marchesi S, Wykowska A. Robot faces that follow gaze facilitate attentional engagement
and increase their likeability. Frontiers in psychology. 2018 Feb 5; 9:70. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.
2018.00070 PMID: 29459842
13. Dautenhahn K. Robots as social actors: Aurora and the case of autism. InProc. CT99, The Third Inter-
national Cognitive Technology Conference, August, San Francisco 1999 Aug (Vol. 359, p. 374).
14. Duquette A, Michaud F, Mercier H. Exploring the use of a mobile robot as an imitation agent with chil-
dren with low-functioning autism. Autonomous Robots. 2008 Feb; 24(2):147–57.
15. van Straten CL, Smeekens I, Barakova E, Glennon J, Buitelaar J, Chen A. Effects of robots’ intonation
and bodily appearance on robot-mediated communicative treatment outcomes for children with autism
spectrum disorder. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing. 2018 Apr; 22(2):379–90.
16. Costescu CA, Vanderborght B, David DO. Reversal learning task in children with autism spectrum dis-
order: a robot-based approach. Journal of autism and developmental disorders. 2015 Nov; 45
(11):3715–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2319-z PMID: 25479815
17. Silva K, Lima M, Santos-Magalhães A, Fafiães C, de Sousa L. Can dogs assist children with severe
autism spectrum disorder in complying with challenging demands? An exploratory experiment with a
live and a robotic dog. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine. 2018 Mar 1; 24
(3):238–42. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2017.0254 PMID: 29116816
18. Silva K, Lima M, Santos-Magalhães A, Fafiães C, de Sousa L. Living and robotic dogs as elicitors of
social communication behavior and regulated emotional responding in individuals with Autism and
severe language delay: A preliminary comparative study. Anthrozoo
¨
s. 2019 Jan 2; 32(1):23–33.
19. Costa AP, Charpiot L, Lera FR, Ziafati P, Nazarikhorram A, Van Der Torre L, et al. More attention and
less repetitive and stereotyped behaviors using a robot with children with autism. In2018 27th IEEE
International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN) 2018 Aug 27
(pp. 534–539). IEEE.
20. Srinivasan SM, Kaur M, Park IK, Gifford TD, Marsh KL, Bhat AN. The effects of rhythm and robotic inter-
ventions on the imitation/praxis, interpersonal synchrony, and motor performance of children with
autism spectrum disorder (ASD): a pilot randomized controlled trial. Autism research and treatment.
2015 Oct; 2015.
21. Srinivasan SM, Park IK, Neelly LB, Bhat AN. A comparison of the effects of rhythm and robotic interven-
tions on repetitive behaviors and affective states of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD).
Research in autism spectrum disorders. 2015 Oct 1; 18:51–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.07.
004 PMID: 26251668
22. Sarrica M, Brondi S, Fortunati L. How many facets does a “social robot” have? A review of scientific and
popular definitions online. Information Technology & People. 2019 Apr 12.
23. Baraka K, Alves-Oliveira P, Ribeiro T. An extended framework for characterizing social robots. InHu-
man-Robot Interaction 2020 (pp. 21–64). Springer, Cham.
24. Kaiser MD. Social perception and social abilities: Relationships between action perception and autistic
traits. Rutgers The State University of New Jersey-Newark; 2010.
25. Henschel A, Laban G, Cross ES. What makes a robot social? a review of social robots from science fic-
tion to a home or hospital near you. Current Robotics Reports. 2021 Mar; 2(1):9–19. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s43154-020-00035-0 PMID: 34977592
26. Dawe J, Sutherland C, Barco A, Broadbent E. Can social robots help children in healthcare contexts? A
scoping review. BMJ paediatrics open. 2019; 3(1). https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjpo-2018-000371 PMID:
30815587
27. Hedges SH, Odom SL, Hume K, Sam A. Technology use as a support tool by secondary students with
autism. Autism. 2018 Jan; 22(1):70–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361317717976 PMID: 28936882
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 21 / 25
28. Maglione MA, Gans D, Das L, Timbie J, Kasari C. Nonmedical interventions for children with ASD: Rec-
ommended guidelines and further research needs. Pediatrics. 2012 Nov 1; 130(Supplement_2):S169–
78. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-0900O PMID: 23118248
29. Mengoni SE, Irvine K, Thakur D, Barton G, Dautenhahn K, Guldberg K, et al. Feasibility study of a ran-
domised controlled trial to investigate the effectiveness of using a humanoid robot to improve the social
skills of children with autism spectrum disorder (Kaspar RCT): A study protocol. BMJ open. 2017 Jun 1;
7(6):e017376. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2017-017376 PMID: 28645986
30. Bellini S, Akullian J. A meta-analysis of video modeling and video self-modeling interventions for chil-
dren and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Exceptional children. 2007 Apr; 73(3):264–87.
31. Rao PA, Beidel DC, Murray MJ. Social skills interventions for children with Asperger’s syndrome or
high-functioning autism: A review and recommendations. Journal of autism and developmental disor-
ders. 2008 Feb; 38(2):353–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-007-0402-4 PMID: 17641962
32. Salimi Z, Jenabi E, Bashirian S. Are social robots ready yet to be used in care and therapy of autism
spectrum disorder: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews. 2021 Oct 1; 129:1–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.04.009 PMID: 33862066
33. Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, et al. Preferred reporting items for
systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic reviews. 2015
Dec; 4(1):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1186/2046-4053-4-1 PMID: 25554246
34. Borenstein M, Hedges LV, Higgins JP, Rothstein HR. Introduction to meta-analysis. John Wiley &
Sons; 2021 Apr 6.
35. Armijo-Olivo S, Stiles CR, Hagen NA, Biondo PD, Cummings GG. Assessment of study quality for sys-
tematic reviews: a comparison of the Cochrane Collaboration Risk of Bias Tool and the Effective Public
Health Practice Project Quality Assessment Tool: methodological research. Journal of evaluation in
clinical practice. 2012 Feb; 18(1):12–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2753.2010.01516.x PMID:
20698919
36. Kim ES, Daniell CM, Makar C, Elia J, Scassellati B, Shic F. Potential clinical impact of positive affect in
robot interactions for autism intervention. In2015 International Conference on Affective Computing and
Intelligent Interaction (ACII) 2015 Sep 21 (pp. 8–13). IEEE.
37. Srinivasan SM, Eigsti IM, Gifford T, Bhat AN. The effects of embodied rhythm and robotic interventions
on the spontaneous and responsive verbal communication skills of children with Autism Spectrum Dis-
order (ASD): A further outcome of a pilot randomized controlled trial. Research in autism spectrum dis-
orders. 2016 Jul 1; 27:73–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.04.001 PMID: 27668011
38. So WC, Wong MK, Lam WY, Cheng CH, Yang JH, Huang Y, et al. Robot-based intervention may
reduce delay in the production of intransitive gestures in Chinese-speaking preschoolers with autism
spectrum disorder. Molecular autism. 2018 Dec; 9(1):1–6. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-018-0217-5
PMID: 29796238
39. So WC, Wong MK, Lam CK, Lam WY, Chui AT, Lee TL, Ng HM, Chan CH, Fok DC. Using a social
robot to teach gestural recognition and production in children with autism spectrum disorders. Disability
and Rehabilitation: Assistive Technology. 2018 Aug 18; 13(6):527–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/
17483107.2017.1344886 PMID: 28673117
40. So WC, Cheng CH, Lam WY, Wong T, Law WW, Huang Y, Ng KC, Tung HC, Wong W. Robot-based
play-drama intervention may improve the narrative abilities of Chinese-speaking preschoolers with
autism spectrum disorder. Research in developmental disabilities. 2019 Dec 1; 95:103515. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.ridd.2019.103515 PMID: 31670026
41. So WC, Wong MK, Lam WY, Cheng CH, Ku SY, Lam KY, et al. Who is a better teacher for children with
autism? Comparison of learning outcomes between robot-based and human-based interventions in
gestural production and recognition. Research in developmental disabilities. 2019 Mar 1; 86:62–75.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2019.01.002 PMID: 30677695
42. So WC, Cheng CH, Lam WY, Huang Y, Ng KC, Tung HC, et al. A robot-based play-drama intervention
may improve the joint attention and functional play behaviors of chinese-speaking preschoolers with
autism spectrum disorder: a pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2020 Feb; 50
(2):467–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04270-z PMID: 31655965
43. Kim ES, Berkovits LD, Bernier EP, Leyzberg D, Shic F, Paul R, et al. Social robots as embedded rein-
forcers of social behavior in children with autism. Journal of autism and developmental disorders. 2013
May; 43(5):1038–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1645-2 PMID: 23111617
44. Zheng Z, Nie G, Swanson A, Weitlauf A, Warren Z, Sarkar N. Longitudinal impact of autonomous robot-
mediated joint attention intervention for young children with ASD. International Conference on Social
Robotics 2016 Nov 1 (pp. 581–590). Springer, Cham.
45. Marino F, Chilà P, Sfrazzetto ST, Carrozza C, Crimi I, Failla C, et al. Outcomes of a robot-assisted
social-emotional understanding intervention for young children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 22 / 25
of autism and developmental disorders. 2020 Jun; 50(6):1973–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-
03953-x PMID: 30852783
46. So WC, Wong MY, Cabibihan JJ, Lam CY, Chan RY, Qian HH. Using robot animation to promote ges-
tural skills in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning. 2016
Dec; 32(6):632–46.
47. So WC, Cheng CH, Law WW, Wong T, Lee C, Kwok FY, et al. Robot dramas may improve joint atten-
tion of Chinese-speaking low-functioning children with autism: Stepped wedge trials. Disability and
Rehabilitation: Assistive Technology. 2020 Nov 11:1–0. https://doi.org/10.1080/17483107.2020.
1841836 PMID: 33186058
48. Huskens B, Verschuur R, Gillesen J, Didden R, Barakova E. Promoting question-asking in school-aged
children with autism spectrum disorders: Effectiveness of a robot intervention compared to a human-
trainer intervention. Developmental neurorehabilitation. 2013 Oct 1; 16(5):345–56. https://doi.org/10.
3109/17518423.2012.739212 PMID: 23586852
49. Huskens B, Palmen A, Van der Werff M, Lourens T, Barakova E. Improving collaborative play between
children with autism spectrum disorders and their siblings: The effectiveness of a robot-mediated inter-
vention based on Lego® therapy. Journal of autism and developmental disorders. 2015 Nov; 45
(11):3746–55. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2326-0 PMID: 25428293
50. Kaboski JR, Diehl JJ, Beriont J, Crowell CR, Villano M, Wier K, et al. Brief report: A pilot summer robot-
ics camp to reduce social anxiety and improve social/vocational skills in adolescents with ASD. Journal
of autism and developmental disorders. 2015 Dec; 45(12):3862–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-
2153-3 PMID: 24898910
51. De Korte MW, van den Berk-Smeekens I, van Dongen-Boomsma M, Oosterling IJ, Den Boer JC, Bara-
kova EI, et al. Self-initiations in young children with autism during Pivotal Response Treatment with and
without robot assistance. Autism. 2020 Nov; 24(8):2117–28. https://doi.org/10.1177/
1362361320935006 PMID: 32730096
52. Tapus A, Peca A, Aly A, Pop C, Jisa L, Pintea S, et al. Children with autism social engagement in inter-
action with Nao, an imitative robot: A series of single case experiments. Interaction studies. 2012 Jan 1;
13(3):315–47.
53. Warren ZE, Zheng Z, Swanson AR, Bekele E, Zhang L, Crittendon JA, et al. Can robotic interaction
improve joint attention skills?. Journal of autism and developmental disorders. 2015 Nov; 45(11):3726–
34. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1918-4 PMID: 24014194
54. Zheng Z, Nie G, Swanson A, Weitlauf A, Warren Z, Sarkar N. A randomized controlled trial of an intelli-
gent robotic response to joint attention intervention system. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
orders. 2020 Aug; 50(8):2819–31. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04388-5 PMID: 32026173
55. Pop CA, Simut R, Pintea S, Saldien J, Rusu A, David D, et al. Can the social robot Probo help children
with autism to identify situation-based emotions? A series of single case experiments. International
Journal of Humanoid Robotics. 2013 Sep 1; 10(03):1350025.
56. Pop CA, Simut RE, Pintea S, Saldien J, Rusu AS, Vanderfaeillie J, et al. Social robots vs. computer dis-
play: Does the way social stories are delivered make a difference for their effectiveness on ASD chil-
dren?. Journal of Educational Computing Research. 2013 Oct; 49(3):381–401.
57. Pop CA, Pintea S, Saldien J, Vanderfaeillie, David D. Enhancing play skills, engagement and social
skills in a play task in ASD children by using robot-based interventions. A pilot study. GB: Interaction
studies; 2014. p. 292–320.
58. Simut R, Vanderfaeillie J, Vanderborght B, Pop C, Pintea S, Rusu A, et al. Is the social robot Probo an
added value for Social Story Intervention for children with ASD?. In2012 7th ACM/IEEE International
Conference on Human-Robot Interaction (HRI) 2012 Mar 5 (pp. 235–236). IEEE.
59. Simut RE, Vanderfaeillie J, Peca A, Van de Perre G, Vanderborght B. Children with autism spectrum
disorders make a fruit salad with probo, the social robot: An interaction study. Journal of autism and
developmental disorders. 2016 Jan; 46(1):113–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2556-9 PMID:
26304031
60. Vanderborght B, Simut R, Saldien J, Pop C, Rusu AS, Pintea S, et al. Using the social robot probo as a
social story telling agent for children with ASD. Interaction Studies. 2012 Jan 1; 13(3):348–72.
61. Silva K, Lima M, Fafiães C, Sinval J, de Sousa L. Preliminary test of the potential of contact with dogs to
elicit spontaneous imitation in children and adults with severe autism spectrum disorder. The American
Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2020 Jan; 74(1):7401205070p1–8. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.2020.
031849 PMID: 32078518
62. Puyon M, Giannopulu I. Emergent emotional and verbal strategies in autism are based on multimodal
interactions with toy robots in free spontaneous game play. In2013 IEEE RO-MAN 2013 Aug 26 (pp.
593–597). IEEE.
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 23 / 25
63. Pierno AC, Mari M, Lusher D, Castiello U. Robotic movement elicits visuomotor priming in children with
autism. Neuropsychologia. 2008 Jan 1; 46(2):448–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.
08.020 PMID: 17920641
64. Giannopulu I, Montreynaud V, Watanabe T. Neurotypical and autistic children aged 6 to 7 years in a
speaker-listener situation with a human or a minimalist InterActor robot. InThe 23rd IEEE international
symposium on robot and human interactive communication 2014 Aug 25 (pp. 942–948). IEEE.
65. Costescu CA, Vanderborght B, David DO. ROBOT-ENHANCED CBT FOR DYSFUNCTIONAL EMO-
TIONS IN SOCIAL SITUATIONS FOR CHILDREN WITH ASD. Journal of Evidence-Based Psychother-
apies. 2017 Sep 1; 17(2).
66. Srinivasan S, Bhat A. The effect of robot-child interactions on social attention and verbalization patterns
of typically developing children and children with autism between 4 and 8 years.
67. Yun SS, Choi J, Park SK, Bong GY, Yoo H. Social skills training for children with autism spectrum disor-
der using a robotic behavioral intervention system. Autism Research. 2017 Jul; 10(7):1306–23. https://
doi.org/10.1002/aur.1778 PMID: 28464438
68. Kumazaki H, Yoshikawa Y, Yoshimura Y, Ikeda T, Hasegawa C, Saito DN, et al. The impact of robotic
intervention on joint attention in children with autism spectrum disorders. Molecular autism. 2018 Dec; 9
(1):1–0. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-018-0230-8 PMID: 30202508
69. Scassellati B, Boccanfuso L, Huang CM, Mademtzi M, Qin M, Salomons N, et al. Improving social skills
in children with ASD using a long-term, in-home social robot. Science Robotics. 2018 Aug 22; 3(21):
eaat7544. https://doi.org/10.1126/scirobotics.aat7544 PMID: 33141724
70. Yoshikawa Y, Kumazaki H, Matsumoto Y, Miyao M, Kikuchi M, Ishiguro H. Relaxing gaze aversion of
adolescents with autism spectrum disorder in consecutive conversations with human and android robot
—a preliminary study. Frontiers in psychiatry. 2019: 370. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00370
PMID: 31258488
71. Srinivasan SM, Lynch KA, Bubela DJ, Gifford TD, Bhat AN. Effect of interactions between a child and a
robot on the imitation and praxis performance of typically developing children and a child with autism: A
preliminary study. Perceptual and motor skills. 2013 Jun; 116(3):885–904. https://doi.org/10.2466/15.
10.PMS.116.3.885-904 PMID: 24175461
72. Alcorn AM, Ainger E, Charisi V, Mantinioti S, Petrović S, Schadenberg BR, et al. Educators’ views on
using humanoid robots with autistic learners in special education settings in England. Frontiers in
Robotics and AI. 2019 Nov 1; 6:107. https://doi.org/10.3389/frobt.2019.00107 PMID: 33501122
73. Stahmer AC, Dababnah S, Rieth SR. Considerations in implementing evidence-based early autism
spectrum disorder interventions in community settings. Pediatric medicine (Hong Kong, China). 2019
May;2.
74. Robins B, Dautenhahn K. The role of the experimenter in hri research-a case study evaluation of chil-
dren with autism interacting with a robotic toy. InROMAN 2006-The 15th IEEE International Symposium
on Robot and Human Interactive Communication 2006 Sep 6 (pp. 646–651). IEEE.
75. Aresti-Bartolome N, Garcia-Zapirain B. Technologies as support tools for persons with autistic spectrum
disorder: a systematic review. International journal of environmental research and public health. 2014
Aug; 11(8):7767–802. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph110807767 PMID: 25093654
76. Russell G, Mandy W, Elliott D, White R, Pittwood T, Ford T. Selection bias on intellectual ability in
autism research: A cross-sectional review and meta-analysis. Molecular autism. 2019 Dec; 10(1):1–0.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-019-0260-x PMID: 30867896
77. Hashim R, Yussof H. Humanizing humanoids towards social inclusiveness for children with autism. Pro-
cedia Computer Science. 2017 Jan 1; 105:359–64.
78. Chia GL, Anderson A, McLean LA. Use of technology to support self-management in individuals with
Autism: Systematic Review. Review Journal of Autism and developmental disorders. 2018 Jun; 5
(2):142–55.
79. Virnes M, Ka
¨
rna
¨
E, Vellonen V. Review of research on children with autism spectrum disorder and the
use of technology. Journal of Special Education Technology. 2015 Mar; 30(1):13–27.
80. Wong C, Odom SL, Hume KA, Cox AW, Fettig A, Kucharczyk S, et al. Evidence-based practices for
children, youth, and young adults with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehensive review. Journal of
autism and developmental disorders. 2015 Jul; 45(7):1951–66. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-
2351-z PMID: 25578338
81. Zervogianni V, Fletcher-Watson S, Herrera G, Goodwin M, Pe
´
rez-Fuster P, Brosnan M, et al. A frame-
work of evidence-based practice for digital support, co-developed with and for the autism community.
Autism. 2020 Aug; 24(6):1411–22. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319898331 PMID: 32026715
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 24 / 25
82. Kumazaki H, Warren Z, Swanson A, Yoshikawa Y, Matsumoto Y, Takahashi H, et al. Can robotic sys-
tems promote self-disclosure in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder? A pilot study. Frontiers in
Psychiatry. 2018 Feb 9; 9:36. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00036 PMID: 29479324
83. Boccanfuso L, Scarborough S, Abramson RK, Hall AV, Wright HH, O’Kane JM. A low-cost socially
assistive robot and robot-assisted intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder: field trials
and lessons learned. Autonomous Robots. 2017 Mar; 41(3):637–55.
84. Huijnen CA, Lexis MA, Jansens R, de Witte LP. How to implement robots in interventions for children
with autism? A co-creation study involving people with autism, parents and professionals. Journal of
autism and developmental disorders. 2017 Oct; 47(10):3079–96. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-
3235-9 PMID: 28685411
85. Koegel RL, Koegel LK. Pivotal response treatments for autism: Communication, social, & academic
development. Paul H Brookes Publishing; 2006.
86. Dawson-Squibb JJ, de Vries PJ. Developing an evaluation framework for parent education and training
in autism spectrum disorder: Results of a multi-stakeholder process. Journal of Autism and Develop-
mental Disorders. 2019 Nov; 49(11):4468–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04176-w PMID:
31463631
PLOS ONE
The use of social robots with children and young people on the autism spectrum
PLOS ONE | https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0269800 June 22, 2022 25 / 25