The author(s) shown below used Federal funds provided by the U.S.
Department of Justice and prepared the following final report:
Document Title: An Assessment of Defense and Prosecutorial
Strategies in Terrorism Trials: Implications for
State and Federal Prosecutors
Author(s): Chris Shields, Kelly R. Damphousse, Brent L.
Smith
Document No.: 228276
Date Received: September 2009
Award Number: 2006-IJ-CX-0026
This report has not been published by the U.S. Department of Justice.
To provide better customer service, NCJRS has made this Federally-
funded grant final report available electronically in addition to
traditional paper copies.
Opinions or points of view expressed are those
of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect
the official position or policies of the U.S.
Department of Justice.
N
I
F
A
n
St
I
m
C
h
Te
U
n
K
e
U
n
B
r
Te
U
n
D
e
I
J Grant 20
0
INAL
n
Assess
m
rate
g
ies i
n
m
plication
s
h
ris Shields
rrorism Res
e
n
iversity of
e
ll
y
R. Dam
p
n
iversity of
O
r
ent L. Smi
t
rrorism Res
e
n
iversity of
A
e
cember 200
0
6-IJ-CX-0
REPO
R
m
ent of De
f
n
Terrori
s
s
for Stat
e
e
arch Cente
r
A
rkansas
p
housse
O
klahoma
t
h
e
arch Cente
r
A
rkansas
8
Terroris
m
026
R
T
f
ense and
s
m Trials:
e
and Fed
e
r
in Fulbrig
h
r
in Fulbrig
h
m
Research Cen
t
Prosecut
o
e
ral Pros
e
h
t College
h
t College
t
er in Fulbright
C
o
rial
e
cutors
C
ollege
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
ii
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY
An Assessment of Defense and
Prosecutorial Strategies in Terrorism Trials:
Implications for State and Federal Prosecutors
After the attacks of September 11, 2001, the Department of Justice published several
reports claiming success in prosecuting terrorists (e.g., DOJ, 2006). Some academics,
politicians, and government officials challenged those claims.
1
During this time, there was a
dramatic increase in public interest in the outcome of federal terrorism cases, and an increase in
the level of attention paid to how the government handles cases.
2
Determining whether
government has been successful in the war on terrorism is difficult because defendants accused
of “terrorism-related” offenses are tried in numerous federal district courts and state courts each
year. Few prosecutors have ever faced a politically motivated offender at trial. Those who have,
found themselves responding to persons and issues that were very different from those faced in
traditional trials. Other than findings from the American Terrorism Study (Smith and
Damphousse 1996; 1998; 2003; Smith et al., 2002), little empirical information has been
available to provide guidance relative to the prosecution of these offenders.
Existing research, which primarily consisted of terrorism cases filed before 9/11,
indicated that federal criminal cases involving terrorism defendants differ from other criminal
cases in significant ways.
3
For example, defendants who were labeled terrorist were more likely
to go to trial than non-terrorists, and terrorist defendants were sentenced to significantly longer
1
See, for example, GAO, 2003; and Eggen and Tate, 2005.
2
Different media outlets have levied charges alleging prosecutorial misconduct (Detroit Free Press, 2006) and
witness coaching (NPR, 2006).
3
See, for example, Smith and Orvis 1993; Smith 1994; Smith and Damphousse 1996; 1998; Smith et al., 2002.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
iii
prison sentences than similarly situated non-terrorist defendants.
4
Those studies also suggested
that there may be important differences between terrorists and non-terrorists with regard to
characteristics and processing – that is, prosecuting attorneys and defense attorneys have
developed legal strategies unique to federal terrorism cases.
This research project involved an examination of federal criminal court cases (1980 -
2004) that were filed after defendants were referred to U.S. Attorneys by the Federal Bureau of
Investigation following an official terrorism investigation.
5
The study focused on the court
room processes and legal strategies that were used by federal prosecutors and criminal defense
attorneys from the moment of indictment until the case reached final disposition. Analyses of
these events provide state and federal prosecutors with information to assist them in the efficient
prosecution of terrorism cases. This research included an examination of the relationships
between prosecutorial and defense strategies for the purpose of determining their relationship to
case outcomes in terrorism trials. This study also included an analysis of pre- and post-9/11
federal terrorism cases to determine whether terrorism prosecutions have been more or less
successful in the post-9/11 era.
METHOD
To accomplish these goals, data for this project were extracted from several sources: (1)
the “American Terrorism Study” (ATS), which includes a statistical database of federal
4
Smith and Damphousse, 1996; 1998; Damphousse and Shields, 2007; Bradley, Damphousse and Smith, 2008
5
Lists of terrorist defendants were compiled by the FBI upon request of either the House Judiciary Subcommittee on
Crime or the Senate Judiciary Committee and forwarded to ATS project personnel for further data collection on each
case. The lists include only those federal indictments resulting from investigation by the FBI for terrorism-related
activities under the “terrorism enterprise” section of the Attorney General Guidelines on General Crimes,
Racketeering Enterprises, and Domestic Security/Terrorism Investigations and subsequent editions (1983, 1989,
2002) or in the case of international terrorists, those persons indicted in federal courts as a result of investigation
under the Attorney General Guidelines for FBI Foreign Intelligence Collection and Foreign Counter intelligence
Investigations.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
iv
indictments resulting from official FBI terrorism investigations for the period 1980-2004; (2)
federal court case records (indictments, dockets, etc.); and (3) information from other open
sources, such as newspaper accounts of the trials. The ATS database contains information on
over 700 terrorists indicted for 9,633 violations of federal criminal law from 1980-2004.
In addition, a former Assistant U.S. Attorney served as a subject matter expert
6
to
identify important pleadings, motions, and other key events that occur in federal terrorism trials.
One hundred-forty new variables were created to measure those factors. Among the new
variables are measures that track information about the type of defense attorney used. The
database includes variables that measure whether the defendant received bail, and if not, the
reason bail was denied. One set of variables track whether a superseding indictment was filed in
each case, and another set of variables track the number of counts added or dropped from the
original indictment. Counts were coded by statute number and by United States Code Chapter.
These data also track defense motions and their outcomes, for example: defense challenges to
FISA; motions to suppress physical evidence; motions to suppress electronic surveillance;
motions to sever counts; motions to suppress statements, and; an entire range of pro se motions.
Similarly, these new data track prosecution motions and outcomes (e.g. whether CIPA protection
was sought, motions to exclude defense evidence, challenges to defense strategies, etc.). Data
were collected from each case in the ATS database by examining the court case records. The
new variables were then coded and entered into a flat-file database. Analysis focused on the
following two research questions:
1. Is there a relationship between prosecutorial and defense strategies, and if so, what
impact does it have on case outcomes?
2. How did 9/11 impact the way the federal government responds to terrorism?
6
Joe McLean, Former Assistant U.S. Attorney and Head of the Criminal Division (retired 10/04) for the Northern
District of Alabama.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
v
1.
PROSECUTION AND DEFENSE STRATEGIES
Prosecutors sometimes politicize terrorism cases by drawing attention to terrorist
defendants’ ideological beliefs or terrorist group membership. In the most politicized cases,
prosecutors pursue indictments that tie the defendants’ ideological motivation to the elements of
the case. In effect, prosecutors seek to prove that the defendants engaged in a criminal act to
further terrorist goals. In other cases, prosecutors pursue conventional charges that avoid tying
the defendants’ ideological motivation to the elements of a case, but those prosecutors,
nonetheless, attempt to introduce the defendants’ terrorist “affiliation” at some point in the case.
Finally, some prosecutors treat terrorist defendants as conventional criminal defendants, avoiding
any mention of terrorist group affiliation.
As prosecutors developed new strategies to prosecute terrorist defendants, terrorist
defendants (and their defense attorneys) developed counter defense strategies. Some defense
teams used politicized defenses in an effort to portray the government’s pursuit of terrorist
defendants as something akin to a political witch hunt. Other defense teams focused on the
prosecution’s attempt to link the defendants to a terrorist group or ideology, and pursued
strategies designed to disassociate the defendants from group affiliation. Some defense teams
ignored the politicized nature of their cases and pursued a traditional criminal defense. Finally,
some terrorists actually modified their precursor behavior and tactics to frustrate the
investigation and prosecutorial process (Damphousse and Smith, 2004).
Of course, it is important to note that the cause and effect relationship may not be so cut
and dried. It is probably unknowable how variables such as “strength of evidence” and other
contexts might affect decision-making by both the prosecutors and the defendants. A case with
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
vi
very strong evidence (e.g., tape recordings of a conspiracy) likely affect how both prosecutors
and defendants decide how to handle a case. Similarly, a case with high notoriety may be more
politically difficult for a prosecutor to plea bargain. Unfortunately, access to these kinds of data
is currently not available (although we do try to address strength of evidence with a proxy
variable). Still, discovering the extent to which such strategies are used (and measuring the
success of these strategies, alone and in combination) is an important first step in understanding
how terrorist trials work differently from traditional criminal trials. To the extent that terrorism-
related crimes are tried in criminal courts (cite Chermak and Freilich, criminology and public
policy 2009, forthcoming), these findings have important implications for both state and federal
prosecutors.
Key Findings
The more politicized the prosecution strategy, the more likely the case will go to trial
and the more likely it will result in acquittal or dismissal.
Treating terrorist defendants like traditional offenders results in the highest plea and
conviction rates.
The most explicitly politicized prosecution strategies double the likelihood of acquittal
and dismissal.
Highly politicized defense strategies are associated with an increase the likelihood of
conviction.
To measure the impact of prosecutorial strategies, this study examined prosecutorial
method. This variable was coded in three categories (see Table 1). The first category,
conventional criminality, involves cases in which defendants were charged with conventional
criminal charges and the prosecution made no attempt to link the defendants to a terrorist
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
vii
organization or a terrorist act. Slightly over 20 % of the defendants were prosecuted using this
approach. The next category, political innuendo, is composed of cases in which defendants were
indicted on conventional criminal charges and the prosecutor linked the defendants, expressly or
impliedly, to a terrorist group or ideology. The final and most widely used category was explicit
politicality. It involves counts that draw into question the defendants’ motives for committing a
crime (e.g. sedition, conspiracy to murder, etc.). Typically, defendants in these cases are
publicly linked to a terrorist group. Prosecutors chose this method to use against defendants
slightly over half the time.
Table 1: Prosecution Methods Used
Prosecution Methods
Number of
Defendants Percent
Conventional criminality
149 21.2
Political innuendo
170 24.2
Explicit politicality
385 54.6
Total
704 100.0
Similar to prosecution strategy variables, this study examined defense method. Once
again, three basic strategies emerged. The most frequently used method (about 45%) involved
situations where defense attorneys used a traditional criminal defense. Slightly over one fourth
of the defendants used a second method, disassociation, in an attempt to distance themselves
from other members and/or an ideology. Finally, about 17 % of the defendants chose to claim
that they were innocent and being prosecuted because of their political and/or religious beliefs.
Additional defense strategies were identified, but analyses were conducted with the three most
common, listed below. Table 2 provides the frequency distribution for defense strategies.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
viii
Table 2: Frequency Distribution: Defense strategies used
Defense Methods
Number of
Defendants Percent
Political persecution
120 17.0
Disassociation
179 25.4
Traditional
318 45.2
Other strategies used
87 12.4
Total
704 100.0
As Table 3 below shows, the overall conviction rate (either through guilty plea or trial
conviction) for defendants in federal terrorism trials is approximately 84 %. That is over10 %
lower than traditional federal criminal cases. Portraying defendants as conventional criminals
approximates the national conviction rates in other federal criminal cases. However, it appears
that as more prosecutors politicize these cases, their conviction rates drop. The relationship
between prosecutorial and defense strategies is situational. The results also indicate that defense
strategies do not have as much of an impact on overall conviction rates, with one possible
exception. The conviction rate for highly politicized defenses tended to be higher than the model
average. While some strategies produce higher conviction rates and others produce lower
conviction rates, the effect of how much lower or higher depends on the combination. Analyses
showed that, despite being the most common combination of prosecution strategy and defense
strategy, conventional criminality and traditional defense did not produce the highest conviction
rates. The combination of a conventional criminal prosecution strategy and either the
dissociation defense method, or the political persecution defense method produced the highest
conviction rates. Conventional criminality produced the highest conviction rates among all
prosecution strategies. Political innuendo was slightly less successful, overall, than conventional
criminality.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
ix
Table 3: Frequency Table of Prosecution Strategy and
Defense Strategy Conviction Percentages
Prosecution Strategy
Overall
conviction
percentage by
defense
strategy
Conventional
Criminality
Political
Innuendo
Explicit
Politicality
Defense
Strategy
Political
Persecution
5/5*
100% (conv rate)
29/30
96.7 %
57/68
83.8%
91/103
88.3%
Disassociation
2/2
100 %
31/37
83.7%
97/122
79.5%
130/161
80.7%
Traditional
Defense
117/127
92.1%
45/51
88.2%
82/115
71.3%
244/293
83.2%
Overall conviction percentage
by prosecution strategy
124 /134
92.5%
105/118
88.9%
236/305
77.4%
485/577
84.1%
* #convictions / #cases
Even though the conventional criminality prosecution strategy proved to be the most
successful strategy overall, it did so only in a handful of cases where defendants used either the
political innuendo or dissociation defense strategies. The political innuendo prosecution
strategy, when used in cases relying on the political persecution defense produced the highest,
statistically reliable results. The results suggest that this outcome was most likely due to the
political persecution defense strategy, which had a positive effect on the probability of
conviction. The lowest conviction rate occurred in the combination of explicit politicality
prosecution strategy and traditional defense strategy.
In fact, among all prosecution strategies, explicit politicality produced the lowest
conviction rate (77.4%). Similarly, the disassociation defense strategy produced the lowest
conviction rates among all defense strategies (80.7%). Explicit politicality and a traditional
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
x
defense strategy was the combination that resulted in the lowest conviction rate. That trend did
not continue across the different combinations. The findings showed that the disassociation
defense strategy was more successful than the traditional defense strategy when used against the
political innuendo prosecution strategy. Most likely this result occurred because of the nature of
political innuendo cases and the amount and type of evidence that is used to link defendants to
terrorism.
The results indicate that using the explicit politicality prosecution strategy presents
prosecutors with the biggest challenge for gaining convictions. The findings produced a
statistically significant negative effect on the likelihood of conviction even when the impact of
evidentiary strength, case complexity, and count severity are controlled. Likewise, defendants
who use the political persecution strategy, regardless of prosecution method, are statistically
more likely to be convicted than when using an alternative defense strategy.
A Note About Seditious Conspiracy
Terrorism cases that have been explicitly politicized sometimes involve seditious
conspiracy charges under 18 USC §2384, a statute that has been used infrequently over the last
60 years outside of terrorism cases. Between 1983 and 2004 thirty-eight persons indicted in FBI
terrorism investigations included charges of seditious conspiracy. Such charges, by their very
nature, result in explicitly politicizing the trial process and limiting prosecutorial options. Cases
in which these charges occurred involved: (1) the trial of four FALN members in Chicago in
1983; (2) the 1988 trial of thirteen members of extreme right wing groups tried in Fort Smith,
Arkansas, ten of whom were charged with seditious conspiracy; (3) the 1989 trial of the eight
members of the United Freedom Front in Boston; (4) the 1995 case involving fifteen defendants
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xi
in the New York City landmarks bombing conspiracy; and (5) the 2005 conviction of Ali al-
Timimi for his role in leading the Virginia Jihad Network.
These cases stand out due to the contrast in convictions rates for “domestic” terrorists
charged with seditious conspiracy compared with “international” terrorists charged with this
offense. In the two cases involving purely domestic groups (the right wing group leaders and the
UFF members), the overwhelming majority were either acquitted of the charges outright or the
charges were dismissed. Of the eighteen defendants charged with seditious conspiracy in these
two cases, only two were convicted (both by early plea). In the Fort Smith case, all ten right
wing group members were acquitted; while in Boston, three of the UFF members saw this charge
dismissed and three were acquitted of it at trial.
Conviction rates in seditious conspiracy cases involving “foreigners” were dramatically
higher. All four members of the FALN in the 1983 case were convicted at trial; thirteen of the
fifteen Islamic extremists in the New York City landmarks case were convicted, ten at trial; and
the leader of the Virginia Jihad Network was also convicted at trial in 2005. In comparison, for
cases involving “international” terrorism, conviction rates for seditious conspiracy stand at 90%
(18 of 20 defendants), while only 12.5% (2 of 16) of domestic terrorists charged with seditious
conspiracy were convicted of this charge.
While it may be argued that American juries are more likely to perceive a greater threat
from “international” terrorists than “homegrown” terrorists, other factors may have contributed
to this disparity in conviction rates. In particular, the defendants in the two left- and right-wing
domestic cases had been previously tried and convicted for crimes that later comprised the overt
acts in the seditious conspiracy charges. This was also true of a 1989 conspiracy case involving
members of the May 19
th
Communist Organization with similar results. In all of these domestic
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xii
cases, jurors (and even one judge) expressed the sentiment that “haven’t these defendants already
been convicted of these crimes.” In contrast, the seditious conspiracy charges against most of the
international terrorists were part of the original indictments in the first cases against these
defendants. Regardless of the relative importance of these issues, both of these factors seem
strongly correlated with conviction rates in seditious conspiracy cases: (1) whether the case
involved “international” versus “homegrown” terrorists, and (2) whether the defendants had been
tried and previously convicted of “overt acts” included in the seditious conspiracy charges.
2. PRE- AND POST-9/11
The events of 9/11 ushered in a number of policy changes that were aimed at improving
U.S. antiterrorism policy. Among the changes that affected the FBI and the Executive Office of
U.S. Attorneys was a mandate issued by former Attorney General John Ashcroft that directed
both agencies to intercept, interrupt and prosecute suspected terrorists before another event like
9/11 could take place.
7
Reports issued by Department of Justice concerning the effectiveness of
these policy changes have been the subject of intense criticism. This study indicates that some of
that criticism may be misplaced.
The findings suggest that the FBI and the EOUSA pursued two types of terrorism-related
cases before 9/11 and added a third type afterwards. Before 9/11 almost all terrorism cases
involved defendants who were linked to a terrorist group or ideology, and a majority of those
defendants were linked to a planned or completed act of terrorism. After 9/11, and consistent
with the new policy changes, the FBI and EOUSA began pursing certain types of criminality in
an effort to diffuse terrorist acts before they could occur. Hence, this new strategy is referred to
as “diffusion.” To target international terrorist groups, officials focused on identification fraud
7
Ashcroft, John, (2002), Ashcroft fact sheet on new FBI investigative guidelines, Politechbot.com, September 21,
2002
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xiii
that could be used to get human assets into the United States, and financial fraud that might be
used to fund terrorist activity both here and abroad.
Key Findings
After 9/11, the government pursued a dramatically smaller proportion of cases that
linked defendants to a group/ideology and a completed/planned act of terrorism (85 %
before 9/11 to 28 % after 9/11).
Plea bargain rates increased 32.7 % while acquittal rates decreased by 6.8 % in the
post-9/11 era—resulting in an overall increase in conviction rates after 9/11.
Prosecutors made a significant shift away from explicitly politicized prosecution
strategies in the post-9/11 era (66.1 % to 25.3%).
Prosecutors treated terrorist defendants like traditional offenders significantly more
often in the post-9/11 era (52.4 %) than in the pre-9/11 era (11.2 %).
After 9/11, in nearly half of all terrorism prosecutions, prosecutors pursued an entirely
new type of terrorism case (diffusion) based in large part on changes mandated in post-
9/11 antiterrorism policy.
Cases in the study were divided by case type using the scheme in Figure 1.
8
The event-
linked category was composed of cases that linked defendants to a terrorist group or ideology
and provided demonstrable links to a planned or completed act of terrorism. Pretextual cases
were those where the government had some reason to suspect defendants were linked to a
terrorist group/ideology, but no evidence linking them to an act of terrorism. In these cases, the
prosecutor pursued any criminal charges that happened to be available as a pretext to get
terrorists off the street. After the 9/11 attacks, public and congressional pressure demanded that
the FBI became proactive with a renewed focus on intelligence gathering and terrorism
prevention. Inevitably, criminal cases emerged out of terrorism investigations that lacked any
8
Robert Chesney (2007) created the scheme for the case type categories, but this research is the first to examine
each in detail.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xiv
known links to terrorism. These are referred to as “diffusion” cases. Diffusion cases also
occurred when the government pursued particular forms of criminality (e.g.
identification/immigration or financial fraud) but lacked evidence that linked defendants to a
particular terrorist threat or ideology. In effect, government officials became engaged in passive-
defense and target-hardening measures. Potential terrorist threats are diffused, so the argument
goes, because terrorist groups routinely engage in certain types of criminality, so cracking down
on everyone interrupts terrorist planning.
Figure 1: Case Type by Terrorism Link
Case Type
Linked to Extremist Group
or Ideology
Linked to a completed or
planned act of terrorism
Event-linked
Defendant(s) linked in
case documents
Defendants Linked in
Case Documents
Pretextual
Defendant(s) linked in
case documents
No Link
Diffusion
No Link No Link
After the case types were established, analyses were performed dividing the cases into
pre- and post-9/11 samples with the year 2001 deleted. Nearly 85 % of the terrorism cases in
the pre-9/11 era were event-linked, with prosecutors pursuing pretextual cases against the
remaining defendants. After 9/11, the proportion of event-linked cases dropped to just 30%
while the percent of pretextual cases increased to 22 %. The results show that the push to be
more proactive had a strong impact on prosecutors, as nearly half of the terrorism cases filed in
the post-9/11 era were diffusion cases. With diffusion cases removed from the analysis, only
57% of the terrorism cases were event-linked, while the percentage of pretextual cases increased
to 43%. This is to be expected. The policy shift ushered in by Attorney General Ashcroft
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xv
refocused law enforcement efforts towards intercepting and interrupting terrorist groups before
those groups could successfully plan an attack. By charging potential terrorists as soon as
criminal violations occurred, logically, there would be less evidence available for prosecutors to
link defendants with terrorist acts.
As the results in Tables 4 and 5 reveal, there was also a change in the prosecution
strategies used between the two eras. Notably, the use of explicit politicality dropped from 66 %
Table 4: Case Type by Prosecution Strategy Pre-9/11
Prosecution Strategy
Case Type
Conventional
criminality
Political
innuendo
Explicit
politicality Total
Event-Linked
24
5.6%
101
23.5%
305
70.9%
430
84.1%
Pretextual
33
40.7%
15
18.5%
33
40.7%
81
15.9%
Total
57
11.2%
116
22.7%
338
66.1%
511
100.0%
X
2
= 88.7, df 2, p < 001
Table 5: Case Type by Prosecution Strategy Post-9/11
Prosecution Strategy
Case Type
Conventional
criminality
Political
innuendo
Explicit
politicality Total
Event-Linked
5
10.0%
15
30.0%
30
60.0%
50
30.1%
Pretextual
11
29.7%
16
43.2%
10
27.1%
37
22.3%
Diffusion
71
89.8%
6
7.6%
2
2.6%
79
47.6%
Total
87
52.4%
37
22.3%
42
25.3%
166
100.0%
X
2
= 98.2, df 4, p < 001
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xvi
to just above 25 %, while the use of political innuendo remained relatively unchanged between
eras. The use of conventional prosecution strategies rose dramatically (from just over 11% to
over 52%). Based on what we now know about the negative effects of using an explicitly
politicized prosecution strategy, this shift accounts, at least partially, for the increased conviction
rate in the post-9/11 era.
Some notable trends emerged within case types. For instance, among event-linked cases,
those most likely to be explicitly politicized before 9/11 (71%), prosecutors used explicit
politicality just 60 % of the time after 9/11. While the overall use of political innuendo as a
prosecution strategy remained virtually unchanged between eras, there was a dramatic increase
in its use among pretextual cases (from 18.5 % to 43 %) and a corresponding decrease in the use
of explicit politicality. Even though political innuendo prosecution strategies had a negative
impact on the likelihood of conviction compared to conventional prosecution strategies, the
effect was smaller than the negative effect of explicit politicality.
After 9/11, prosecutors chose less politicized prosecution strategies than we expected.
We theorize that by pursuing cases sooner, per post-9/11 antiterrorism policy, prosecutors had
less evidence available to prosecute defendants using highly politicized strategies. That would
explain the increased reliance on political innuendo and conventional criminality prosecution
strategies in both event-linked and pretextual cases. It would also partially explain the higher
conviction rate in the post-9/11 era. The diffusion cases we observed in the post-9/11 era also
help explain the increased conviction rate. There were no diffusion cases listed by the FBI prior
to 9/11, yet this category represents almost half of the cases filed afterwards. As Table 5 reveals,
almost 90 % of the defendants in diffusion cases were prosecuted using conventional prosecution
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xvii
strategies. Again, treating defendants as traditional offenders results in the highest conviction
rates.
Taking the diffusion cases out of the post-9/11 sample is important because the resulting
mix of event-linked and pretextual cases closely resembles the composition of cases filed prior to
9/11. This allowed a comparison of similar cases before and after 9/11 while placing diffusion
cases in a category that could be considered separately. The average number of defendants
indicted, per year, in event-linked cases was slightly lower in the post-9/11 era, decreasing from
approximately 22 to 17 annually.
9
The average number of defendants indicted in pretextual cases
increased from 4 to 13 annually. In this study, the total number of defendants who were indicted
in cases that were linked to a terrorist ideology (total of event-linked and pretextual cases)
Table 6: Case Type by Defense Strategy Pre-9/11
Defense Strategy
Case Type
Political
Persecution Disassociation Traditional Total
Event-Linked
88
23.8%
125
33.8%
157
42.4%
370
82.4%
Pretextual
5
6.3%
30
38.0%
44
55.7%
79
17.8%
Total
93
20.7%
155
34.5%
201
44.8%
449
100.0%
X
2
= 12.5, df 2, p = 002
increased slightly from 26 to 30 per year.
10
Once again, the increase in pretextual cases
coupled with less politicized prosecutions strategies probably accounts for much of the increase
in plea bargain rates and conviction rates observed in the post-9/11 era.
9
It should be noted that there are more terrorism cases to be collected from the 2002 to 2004 FBI list (ATS and
PADS). Anecdotally, the proportion of pretextual vis-à-vis event-linked cases should not change dramatically.
10
The total number of event-linked and pretextual cases in the post-9/11 era will increase as the remaining cases are
collected and added to the database, and, anecdotally speaking, the total number of defendants indicted in event-
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xviii
Table 7: Case Type by Defense Strategy Post-9/11
Defense Strategy
Case Type
Political
Persecution Disassociation Traditional Total
Event-Linked
7
20.6%
8
23.5%
19
55.9%
34
Pretextual
12
32.4%
6
16.2%
19
51.4%
37
Diffusion
1
1.4%
5
6.8%
68
91.9%
74
Total
20
13.8%
19
13.1%
106
73.1%
145
100.0%
X
2
= 31.5, df 4, p < 001
Analyses of defense strategies produced a few noteworthy trends. First, defendants relied on
traditional defense strategies at a higher rate after 9/11 than before (73 % compared to 44 %),
and the use of political persecution defense strategies decreased overall, with one notable
exception (see Tables 6 and 7). The proportion of defendants using political persecution defense
strategies increased in the post-9/11 era among defendants in pretextual cases. Recall again,
compared to the other defense strategies analyzed, political persecution increased the odds of
conviction.
CONCLUSIONS
The results indicate that prosecutors who explicitly politicize terrorism cases faced the
biggest challenge for gaining convictions. The findings produced a statistically significant
linked cases after 9/11 will remain close to the average for the pre-9/11 era. Similarly, we expect the average
number of defendants indicted for pretextual cases to climb slightly higher than the figures reported.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xix
negative effect on the likelihood of conviction even when the impact of evidentiary strength,
case complexity, and count severity are controlled. Ironically, defendants who explicitly
politicize their trials by claiming political persecution are statistically more likely to be
convicted than when using an alternative defense strategy regardless of prosecution method.
Although not always the case, defendant who chose this route did so as a last ditch effort, in the
face of overwhelming evidence and with minimal plea options, to avoid conviction
The analyses of the case types suggest that after 9/11 prosecutors relied less heavily on
highly politicized prosecution strategies and filed fewer terrorism event-linked cases. As a
result, plea bargain rates and conviction rates increased. This occurred despite the finding that
the seriousness of the average count (count severity) in the post-9/11 era was only slightly lower
than it was in the previous era. The decision to file less politically charged counts is probably a
significant factor accounting for higher plea bargain rates and conviction rates in the post-9/11
era. The case type categories provide a useful tool for analyzing antiterrorism policy by allowing
research to be conducted among similar cases. In the aftermath of the major policy changes that
occurred in the months following 9/11, in which policy objectives and goals were reset, the case
type variable exposes the flaws in earlier research which lumped all terrorism cases into one
category and based any findings on pre-9/11 policy goals.
This study revealed that no diffusion cases were filed before 9/11, yet they made up the
greatest proportion of cases filed afterwards. Our analysis suggests that diffusion cases were not
the product of poor record keeping, as some critics have suggested but the result of policy
changes implemented after 9/11. Attorney General Ashcroft directed the FBI and the EOUSA to
prosecute cases sooner for the purpose of interrupting terrorists before they could complete
attacks. Due to constraints caused by prosecuting cases sooner, prosecutors attempted to diffuse
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xx
potential terrorism threats by targeting crimes that they determined were precursors to terrorism.
Perhaps the most important benefit of using case type to isolate diffusion cases, is that it is
possible to empirically evaluate similar cases before and after 9/11 providing a more reliable
measure of antiterrorism policy.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xxi
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Executive Summary ........................................................................................................ ii
List of Tables ............................................................................................................... xxi
Chapter 1: Introduction .................................................................................................. 1
1.Organization of the Report ..................................................................................... 5
Chapter 2: Review of Relevant Literature ..................................................................... 7
1. The Relationship Between Defense and Prosecutorial Strategies ........................ 7
2. The Problem of Defining Terrorism ................................................................... 11
3. Our Approach to Defining Terrorism ................................................................. 16
4. Context: U.S. Antiterrorism Policy ..................................................................... 18
5. Theoretical Foundations ...................................................................................... 20
5.a. Structural Contextual Theory ........................................................................ 21
5.b. Trickle-up/Hydraulic Effect .......................................................................... 22
6. Research Questions and Hypothesis ................................................................... 23
Research Question 1 ............................................................................................... 25
Research Question 2 .............................................................................................. 27
Chapter 3: Data and Methods ....................................................................................... 29
1. ATS Data Description ......................................................................................... 30
2. Prosecution and Defense Strategies Variables .................................................... 35
3.PADS Data Description ....................................................................................... 36
4. Case Type............................................................................................................ 45
5. Research Question One ....................................................................................... 48
6. Research Question Two ...................................................................................... 50
Chapter 4: Findings For Two Research Questions ....................................................... 54
1. Research Question One ....................................................................................... 54
2. Research Question Two ...................................................................................... 61
2.a. Case Type Descriptive Statistics ...................................................................... 61
2.b. Tests of Hypotheses ...................................................................................... 71
Chapter 5: Prosecutorial and Defense Strategies in Terrorism Cases ........................... 79
1.GeneralProsecution Strategies ............................................................................. 79
2. GeneralDefense Strategies .................................................................................. 86
3. The Relationship Between Prosecutorial and Defense Strategies ...................... 91
4. Summary ............................................................................................................. 97
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xxii
Chapter 6: How 9/11 Changed Prosecutorial and Defense Strategies
used in Federal Terrorism Cases ............................................................... 99
1. Case Type.......................................................................................................... 100
2. Pretextual and Event-linked Cases .................................................................... 103
3. Explicit Politicality in Pretextual and Event-Linked Cases .............................. 104
4. Political Innuendo in Pretextual and Event-Linked Cases ................................ 105
5. Defense Strategies in Pretextual and Event-linked Cases ................................. 107
6. Diffusion Cases ................................................................................................. 108
7. Case Characteristics Before and After 9/11 ...................................................... 111
8. Theoretical Implications and New Concepts .................................................... 116
9. Summary ........................................................................................................... 118
Chapter 7: Policy Implications and Conclusion.......................................................... 121
1. Selected Policy Changes Since 9/11 ................................................................. 121
2.Review of Research Question and Background ................................................. 128
3.Review of Research Question One: Prosecutorial and Defense Strategies ....... 129
4.Review of Research Question Two: Effect of 9/11 ........................................... 131
5. Suggested Future Research ............................................................................... 133
References ................................................................................................................... 135
Appendix 1: Count Severity Codes............................................................................. 139
Appendix 2: Average Count Severity by Prosecution Strategy .................................. 140
Appendix 3: Percentage of Conspiracy Counts by Prosecution Strategy ................... 141
Appendix 4: Crosstab for Count Outcome by Defense Strategy ................................ 142
Appendix 5: Average Number of Counts Dropped per Defendant ............................. 143
Appendix 6: Average Count Severity by Defense Strategy ........................................ 144
Appendix 7: Counts Charged in Diffusion Cases by USC Chapter ............................ 145
Appendix 8: Average Number of Pro Se Motions per Right-wing Case .................... 146
Appendix 9: Crosstab Case-type by Case Outcome 5th Category Added .................. 147
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xxiii
LIST OF TABLES
Table 3.01 Frequency Distribution: ATS Race ............................................................ 30
Table 3.01 Size of Pre- and Post9/11 Samples ............................................................. 31
Table 3.03 Frequency Distribution: Group Type .......................................................... 32
Table 3.04 Frequency Distribution: Defendant Linked to a Target .............................. 33
Table 3.05 Frequency Distribution: Case Outcomes .................................................... 33
Table 3.06 Frequency Distribution: Prosecution methods used ................................... 37
Table 3.07 Frequency Distribution: Defense methods used ........................................ 38
Table 3.08 Frequency Distribution: Bail ...................................................................... 39
Table 3.09 Frequency Distribution: Reason bail was denied ....................................... 40
Table 3.10 Frequency Distribution: Attorney Type ...................................................... 41
Table 3.11 Level Assistance Provided by Confidential Informants ............................. 42
Table 3.12 Level Assistance Provided by Government Agents ................................... 43
Table 3.13 Frequency Distribution: Motion to suppress evidence ............................... 44
.
Table 3.14 Frequency Distribution: Motion to suppress statements ............................. 45
Table 3.15 Frequency Distribution: Case Type ............................................................ 47
.
Table 3.16 Select If Coding Table ................................................................................ 48
Table 4.01 Frequency Table of Prosecution Strategy and defense
Strategy Conviction Percentages ................................................................ 56
Table 4.02 Coding Scheme for Prosecution Strategy ................................................... 58
Table 4.03 Coding Scheme for Defense Strategy ......................................................... 58
Table 4.04 Logistic Regression for Conviction by prosecutions strategy
and defense strategy .................................................................................... 59
Table 4.05 Logistic Regression for Conviction by prosecutions strategy
and defense strategy .................................................................................... 61
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
xxiv
Table 4.06 Case Type Frequencies pre- and post-9/11 ................................................. 63
Table 4.07 Crosstab Case Type by Outcome Pre-9/11 ................................................. 64
Table 4.08 Crosstab Case Type by Outcome Post-9/11 ............................................... 64
Table 4.09 Crosstab Case Type by Prosecution Strategy Pre-9/11 .............................. 67
Table 4.10 Crosstab Case Type by Prosecution Strategy Post-9/11 ............................. 67
Table 4.11 Crosstab Case Type by Defense Strategy Pre-9/11 .................................... 70
Table 4.12 Crosstab Case Type by Defense Strategy Post-9/11 ................................... 71
Table 4.13 Number Cases Dismissed Pre-and Post-9/11 .............................................. 73
Table 4.14 Number Dismissed Counts Per case Pre-and Post-9/11 ............................. 73
Table 4.15 Percentage of Plea Bargain in Pretextual Cases ......................................... 74
Table 4.16 Average Count Severity before and after 9/11 ........................................... 74
Table 4.17 Crosstab Defendants in Pretextual Cases .................................................... 75
Table 4.18 Crosstab Pretextual Cases .......................................................................... 76
Table 4.19 Average Number of Defendants in Event-linked Cases ............................. 76
Table 4.20 Proportion of Cases with a Confidential Informant .................................... 77
Table 4.21 Proportion of Cases with Undercover Agents ............................................ 77
Table 4.22 Number of Confidential Informants per Case ............................................. 78
Table 4.23 Average Level of Assistance Provided by Confidential
Informant per Case ...................................................................................... 78
Table 4.24 Conviction Rate by Count Severity ............................................................ 79
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
1
I: INTRODUCTION
In the years following the attacks of September 11, 2001, the Department of Justice
published several articles reporting success in prosecuting terrorists (e.g., DOJ, 2006).
Academics, politicians, and government officials have challenged those claims (e.g., GAO,
2003; Eggen and Tate, 2005). During this same time, public interest in the outcome of federal
terrorism cases increased dramatically, as has the level of attention paid to how the government
handles cases. Amid increased scrutiny, charges arose alleging prosecutorial misconduct
(Detroit Free Press, 2006); witness coaching (NPR, 2006); and unexpectedly, some communities
demonstrated support for alleged terrorist defendants (Seattle Times, 2006). Following the events
of 9/11 and the subsequent implementation of the USA PATRIOT Act, public interest in the
manner in which terrorist defendants are investigated, detained, and prosecuted has increased
dramatically. Similarly, prosecutors at both the state and federal level have been called upon to
prepare for a significant increase in the prosecution of “politically motivated” offenders.
Unfortunately, little empirical information has been available to guide these efforts. Recent
research has revealed that terrorist defendants behave very differently than traditional defendants
at trial, most revealed by their propensity to not plead guilty (Smith and Orvis 1993; Smith 1994;
Smith and Damphousse 1996; 1998; Smith et al., 2002). Unfortunately, the identification of
specific defense strategies that may provide prosecutors with information necessary to more
effectively prosecute these cases has not been forthcoming.
For example, we are now well aware that terrorists are much more likely to go to trial
than non-terrorists (Smith and Damphousse, 1998; Smith et al., 2002). We note, of course, that
the guilty plea decision also depends on the prosecutor - who may be less willing to offer “good”
plea bargains because of the evidentiary strength or because of political realities. Still, we focus
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
2
here on the decision by the terrorist defendants because of their unique motivations. Terrorists
may be less likely to accept a plea bargain due to their unwillingness to cooperate with the
federal government whose authority they refuse to acknowledge. In addition, they may prefer a
public trial that will expand the reach of their claims of injustice. Finally, they may not be able
to reconcile their belief in their work as “freedom fighting” with having committed a crime.
Furthermore, we have learned that changes in terrorist tactics, such as the implementation
of leaderless resistance by extreme right wing groups, have affected the charges with which these
defendants are accused, as well as their decision-making, during the adjudication process
(Damphousse and Smith, 2003). These findings, however, underscore our lack of understanding
about the dynamics of these events. For example, how do terrorist demands for trial affect the
manner in which prosecutors should pursue these cases? Does greater “commitment” among
“lone wolf” terrorists affect the manner in which these terrorist defendants present themselves at
trial or the types of motions they file in defense of their conduct?
This project examined these and other questions specifically related to how prosecutors
depict terrorist defendants at trial, how terrorist defendants attempt to portray themselves, and
their use of various motions to achieve this end. In particular, we were interested in
understanding whether these attempts affect case outcomes or conviction rates. The project
involved an examination of the federal court case records of criminal cases stemming from FBI
investigations from 1980-2004 where an “official” terrorism investigation was conducted under
the auspices of the Attorney General’s Guidelines for terrorism investigations.
11
The findings in
11
This method should not be confused with counting persons investigated for “terrorism-related” activities using
either the FBI or Executive Office of U.S. Attorneys classification scheme. For reference on the latter, see “Better
Management Oversight and Internal Controls Needed to Ensure Accuracy of Terrorism-Related Statistics,”
Washington, DC: Government Accounting Office, 2003.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
3
this project should be useful for both state and federal prosecutors in understanding how terrorist
defendants are likely to react to indictment and what methods they will use to defend themselves.
The ultimate goal of the project was to provide state and federal prosecutors with
information that could assist in the efficient prosecution of terrorism cases. Findings from the
project and on-line access to selected court documents in previous terrorism cases should be of
considerable utility to both state and federal prosecutors. For example, right-wing terrorists
associated with the “common law courts” movement
12
have filed almost identical arguments
relative to constitutional authority and jurisdiction in several federal court cases. Government
responses, however, have varied from case to case. Understanding the strategies used by
terrorists in their defense (in addition to having on-line access to copies of defense motions used
by various terrorist defendants and the government’s response to those motions) will increase
prosecutorial success rates while reducing expenses associated with these trials. To accomplish
this goal, three objectives were accomplished.
First, we assessed the relationship between prosecutorial and defense strategies and case
outcomes in terrorism trials. Although some previous research suggests that prosecutors are
more apt to be successful (and efficient) using a strategy that depicts terrorists as conventional
criminals, little information has been available relative to how terrorists or their defense
attorneys attempt to portray themselves to the judge and jury. Preliminary data available from
the American Terrorism Study indicate that a significant relationship exists between how
terrorists attempt to portray themselves during trial proceedings and the trial outcome (Smith et
al., 2005). The strategies used by defense lawyers in these cases appear to be associated with the
filing of various types of motions, particularly in limine filings, to prevent prosecutors from
12
This includes the sovereignty movement, jural society movement, and certain anti-tax groups such as Sheriff’s
Posse Comitatus.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
4
using certain terms that might depict them as terrorists. Other common motions include
questioning the authority of the court and severance motions aimed at distancing defendants
from more outspoken members of terrorist conspiracies. The relationships among these issues
have never been explored. One goal of the proposed project was to examine these issues in
greater detail. Our fundamental research question for this objective was “What is the
relationship between prosecutorial and defense strategies, and do those strategies affect case
outcomes?”
Second, we provided an overview of changes in terrorism cases since the September 11,
2001 attacks. The FBI has provided to ATS staff a complete list of terrorism cases for the three
year period immediately following the 9/11 attacks. This is the only complete list made
available for public scrutiny and analysis. Data collection on the overwhelmingly majority of
these cases was complete by the start of this project. This project provides an analysis of the
number and demographic characteristics of those indicted, types of federal charges, and the
outcomes of these cases; comparing these patterns to pre-9/11 cases. Our fundamental research
question for this objective was “How has 9/11 impacted the ways the federal government
responds to terrorism?”
Third, the project added variables to the American Terrorism Study (ATS) database that
includes information on prosecutorial and defense strategies for terrorism cases to provide a
complete record for the period 1980-2004. The project director began collecting data on FBI
terrorism cases in 1988 going back to 1980. When NIJ began funding the ATS project in 1999,
new variables relating to prosecutorial and defense strategies were added to the research
protocol. Unfortunately, some of the most important, precedent-setting terrorism cases in
American history occurred in the late-1980s and 1990s. Data collection on additional court
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
5
records on all of these older cases was funded by DHS/MIPT over the past five years. For the
current project, ATS staff review these older cases (1980-1998) and added information on
prosecutorial and defense strategies to the dataset.
1. ORGANIZATION OF THE REPORT
Chapter 2 describes the relevant theory that informs the hypotheses that developed from
our two main research questions. The chapter begins with an explanation of structural contextual
theory and the hydraulic effect and provides justification for why these theoretical approaches
were used in the study. After a literature review, discussion turns to the development of
hypotheses that were used to analyze the research questions. Chapter 3 describes the data and
methodology used to test the hypotheses - the American Terrorism Study (ATS) and the
Prosecutorial and Defense Strategies (PADS) database. The discussion explains how the data
were collected, how variables were coded, and which variables are analyzed to test each
hypothesis. Chapter 3 also includes a discussion of the various statistical techniques that were
used. In addition, general descriptive statistics for the database are provided.
Chapter 4 describes the findings. This chapter is divided into two main sections, each
providing a description of the analyses for the two main research questions. The next two
chapters are split among the two research questions. Chapter 5 provides a discussion of the
findings for research question one: an analysis of the prosecutorial and defense strategies used in
terrorism cases. Included in this chapter is a description of the outcomes of each prosecution
strategy, each defense strategy, and the outcomes that these strategies produced when used in
combination. Chapter 6 follows with a discussion of research question two: How has 9/11
changed the strategies used by prosecutors and defendants in federal terrorism cases, and what
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
6
impact have those strategies had on case outcomes? The chapter ends with a discussion of
theoretical implications. Chapter 7 concludes the study, including a discussion on policy
implications and an outline of suggested future research.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
7
II: REVIEW OF RELEVANT RESEARCH
This chapter begins by discussing generally the relationship between defense and
prosecutorial strategies in the terrorism context. We then turn to a discussion of the problem of
defining terrorism and our solution. The next section provides context for understanding the
relationship between defense and prosecutorial strategies by describing how changes in U.S.
antiterrorism policy have evolved over the past four decades. Discussion centers on two
critiques that have been raised challenging the impact of antiterrorism policy implemented after
9/11 and on the changes made over the past 25 years to the United States Attorney General
Guidelines authorizing terrorism investigations. The chapter then turns to each of the research
questions and provides the hypotheses that emerged from the review of two key theories:
structural-contextual theory and the hydraulic effect.
1. THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN DEFENSE AND PROSECUTORIAL STRATEGIES
Although some research has been initiated on the effect of changing state statutes on
prosecutors’ abilities to respond to terrorism,
13
virtually no research exists that examines the
dynamic nature of terrorism trials. This study examines three issues that have emerged in recent
research on the prosecution of terrorists in the United States. First, findings from the American
Terrorism Study have indicated that: (a) prosecutors have been more successful when terrorist
defendants are portrayed as “conventional” criminals than when depicted as “terrorists” or
“politically motivated” offenders (Smith and Damphousse 1996; 1998b); and (b) that terrorist
defendants who attempt to “disassociate” themselves from the terrorist group and its ideology
have significantly lower conviction rates than those who do not employ this strategy (Smith et
13
Elaine Nugent, “Local Prosecutors Respond to Terrorism: Responsibilities, Priorities, and Challenges,” an NIJ
project funded under the 2003 solicitation.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
8
al., 2005). This research examines these relationships and their interactions in greater detail.
Second, preliminary findings by Smith and Damphousse (2003) indicated that changes in
terrorist tactics have spilled over into how they defend themselves at trial. Third, research has
indicated that unlike traditional offenders in the federal court system, terrorist defendants are
significantly more likely than traditional federal defendants to take their cases to trial (Smith and
Damphousse 2003; Smith et al. 2002). These differences are so dramatic that examination of the
plea process is warranted for this group of “crime-specific” offenders.
Terrorist groups are not static. They constantly learn new tactics in an effort to increase
the destructive power of their weaponry and to develop more effective ways to avoid detection,
arrest, and prosecution. Part of this process involves efforts by group leaders to insulate
themselves from civil and criminal liability through the development of new organizational
structures. New models, like “leaderless resistance,” have emerged to combat the prosecutorial
successes of the past. Terrorists have learned, primarily by word of mouth, which techniques
work best and which are doomed to failure. Despite the importance of understanding the
changing dynamics of this phenomenon, empirical examination of these issues has been virtually
nonexistent.
When prosecuting terrorists, research has indicated that prosecutors typically begin by
choosing between one of two polar types. They may engage in what Turk (1982) refers to as
“exceptional vagueness” by advocating to the jury that these defendants are nothing more than
“common” criminals. In contrast, they may choose the opposite extreme and attempt to
“explicitly politicize” their conduct (Turk 1982; Smith 1994). These strategies are reflected not
only in the language used during the course of the trial, but also in charging decisions. With the
exception of the highly publicized seditious conspiracy trial of the 1993 World Trade Center
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
9
bombers, federal prosecutors in the 1990s were much more likely to depict terrorists as
conventional criminals.
14
Compared with the prosecution of domestic terrorists, prosecutors have been much more
likely to politicize the indictment and trials of international terrorists, particularly since the
September 11, 2001 attacks (Smith et al., 2002). Frequently, prosecutors moderate these
extremes during the course of the trial by using what Smith and Damphousse (1998b) have
described as “subtle innuendo” – comments intended to “hint” to jurors that these persons, while
committing conventional crimes, are more committed ideologically, and hence, of greater danger
to the public. Generally, though, prosecutors have had greater conviction success by portraying
terrorists as traditional offenders.
In contrast, little is known about the strategies used by defense counselors in terrorism
cases. Does the manner in which terrorist defendants attempt to portray themselves affect the
outcome of their cases? A preliminary examination of data collected on 1990s terrorism cases
revealed that 40% of defendants who attempted to “disassociate” themselves from the group and
its ideology were either acquitted, the case resulted in mistrial, or all charges were dismissed
(Smith et al., 2005). How this effort was accomplished varied from case to case, but it
frequently entailed the use of severances and motions in limine to restrict prosecutors from using
language linking the defendant to specific terrorist groups or ideological concepts.
Some of the interactions of these various efforts by the prosecution and defense are
depicted in Figure 1. It appears that avoiding the issue of “political motive” benefits both the
prosecution and the defense. The irony of this apparent contradiction suggests that such
oversimplification is not only unwarranted, but unwise and potentially costly as well.
14
USA v. Salameh, et al. (93-CR-180), U.S. District Court, Southern District of New York; USA v. Elgabrowny et
al. (93-CR-181) U.S. District Court, Southern District of New York.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
10
Figure 2a:
Potential Predictors of Defense and Prosecutorial
Strategies on Case Outcomes
Many questions emerge from this discussion: What combination of defense and
prosecutorial strategies result in the highest conviction rate? Conversely, what combination of
defense and prosecutorial strategies are most likely to result in acquittal, dismissal, or mistrial?
When various strategies are implemented, what types of motions/filings have been most effective
for the prosecution and the defense? For example, have prosecutors been more successful by
allowing defendants to sever cases or try defendants together? What contributes to the success
of “ideological disassociation” for terrorist defendants? Does filing motions in limine to restrict
prosecutorial use of terms that might link the defendant to a particular group or ideology have a
measurable (negative) effect on prosecutorial success?
Our attempts to answer these questions are informed by two theories: structural
contextual theory and the hydraulic effect (both described below). These theories result in
several hypotheses that we tested to more fully understand the relationship between prosecutorial
Defense Strategies:
For example,
Freedom Fighter
Lack of federal authority
Civil disobedience
Etc.
Prosecutorial Strategies:
Explicit Politicality
Subtle innuendo
Conventional Criminality
Defense Motions and
Responses:
Motions in limine
Severances
Provide Governmental
A
ssistance
Prosecutorial Motions and
Government Responses
Motions in Limine
Responses to constitutional
authority
Etc.
Case Outcomes:
Trial Conviction
Guilty Plea
A
cquittal
Dismissal of charges
Mistrial
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
11
and defense strategies that are used during terrorism trials. Before we discuss our hypotheses,
however, it is important to define what we mean by “terrorism.” We provide context for our
study by describing the difficulties (and subsequent controversies) associated with defining
terrorism and our efforts to overcome these obstacles.
2. THE PROBLEM OF DEFINING TERRORISM
Defining terrorism has been the subject of unresolved debate within academia and
political circles (Burgess, 2003). Negative images associated with the term, and the media’s use
of any number of sympathetic concepts (e.g.,freedom fighter) have confounded the matter
(Rapoport, 1977). As Laqueur (1987:149) stated, “even if there were an objective, value-free
definition of terrorism, covering all its important aspects and features, it would still be rejected
by some for ideological reasons.” Ideological objections have prevented the UN General
Assembly from establishing a clearly stated definition. Until 2001, the Security Council had
adopted a range of measures addressing terrorist threats to peace and security without defining
the term. After September 2001, the Security Council adopted measures against terrorism that
contained serious legal consequences, but again they failed to define the term. Instead the UN
encouraged states to define terrorism in their national bodies of law, which allowed wide and
divergent variations among definitions (Saul, 2004). The Security Council adopted a non-
binding definition in 2004, but critics claim that it “fails to remedy the serious difficulties caused
by the lack of an operative definition in Council practice” (Saul, 2004:41).
Defining terrorism in the United States is complicated as well. Each division of the
government uses its own definition. For example, the Department of Defense defines terrorism
as: “The calculated use of unlawful violence to inculcate fear, intended to coerce or to intimidate
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
12
governments or societies in the pursuit of goals that are generally political, religious, or
ideological”(U.S. Department of Defense, 2003:1). The Department of State defines terrorism as:
“[P]remeditated, politically motivated violence perpetrated against noncombatant targets by
subnational groups or clandestine agents, usually intended to influence an audience” (Office of
the Coordinator for Counterterrorism, 2002:4).
Each of those definitions has several factors in common with most definitions of
terrorism: the terrorists’ motives, methods, and targeting noncombatants. But not all scholars
agree on those concepts, so the debate continues (Burgess, 2003). Some scholars argue that
political motive is too limiting because some individuals act for criminal or religious reasons.
Stern (1999) argues that any definition of terrorism should be unlimited with regard to
perpetrator and purpose. Stern’s approach does not exclude political goals as a terrorist aim, it
simply allows for other motivations. The “deliberate evocation of dread is what sets terrorism
apart from simple murder or assault” (Stern,1999:11). The assumption that terrorists are
motivated to create a psychological impact among their victims has also been debated, as some
attacks could easily be attributed to revenge (Gueke, 1998).
Each of these debates illustrates the difficulty inherent in conceptualizing terrorism, and
perhaps as important, it sheds some light on how onerous the task of theorizing, identifying,
gathering, and measuring data can be regarding terrorism research. Considering the vast amount
of resources that are being directed to fight the “war on terror” in Iraq, in Afghanistan, and
through the creation of the Department of Homeland Security, the importance of reliable
research is more important than ever, but scholarly and political agreement on a universal
definition of terrorism is not imminent.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
13
Due in part to the lack of a universal definition, antiterrorism policy is frustratingly
difficult to evaluate, and the effectiveness of domestic terrorism policy has been the subject of a
lot of controversy and criticism since 9/11. Two years after passage of the USA PATRIOT Act,
officials working for the United States government recognized that American antiterrorism
policy was in need of a major overhaul (Perl, 2003). Policy programs put in place in the wake of
the September 11 attacks lacked any specific requirements for the FBI or Executive Office of
United States Attorneys (“EOUSA”) to develop mechanisms for evaluation. Those that have
been put in place (Zeus in the FBI, and LION in the EOUSA) have proven problematic. The
GAO issued a report in 2003 critical of the EOUSA and the FBI. The report mirrored the data
validity critique that had been appearing in the media for over a year. Researchers argued that
the DOJ included cases in its annual accountability report that should not have been classified
“terrorist” because no link to terrorism appeared in the case documents (Fazlollah & Nicholas,
2001). The GAO found that each unit’s internal mechanisms for reporting terrorism-related
statistics were decidedly inaccurate—overestimating some measures and underestimating others
(GAO-03-266).
Oddly, even after the GAO found the FBI’s reporting more accurate than the EOUSA’s,
the DOJ opted to begin using the EOUSA data (GAO-04-411). The Office of the Inspector
General (“OIG”) issued a report in 2007 finding unresolved problems. The OIG claimed that
DOJ “components did not accurately report terrorism-related statistics in their annual budgets,
financial statements, performance plans, and statistical reports. For most statistics we tested, the
component either could not provide support for the numbers reported or could not identify the
terrorism link used to classify statistics as terrorism-related. Some of the statistics were
significantly overstated and some understated. We concluded that the components lacked
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
14
effective internal controls to ensure accurate reporting of terrorism-related statistics” (OIG-07-
04:2).
The data validity is not the only criticism the DOJ has faced. In December 2001, a story
in the Philadelphia Inquirer questioned the reliability of data used by the DOJ and it raised a
new critique—the soft-sentence critique. An example of the soft sentence critique comes from
TRAC at Syracuse. Researchers gathered data on DOJ terrorism referrals for two years prior to
September 11, 2001 and two years following. In December 2003, TRAC released a report of its
findings, concluding that the DOJ was over-reporting how effective it has been in the war on
terrorism (TRAC, 2003). The TRAC report based this conclusion on findings that more than
half of the referrals received no prison time, and those who received a sentence, received a short
sentence. TRAC found the median sentence for defendant convicted of international terrorism
was 14 days, domestic terrorism was 3 months, and financial terrorism was just under 4 months
(TRAC, 2003; 2006).
Chesney (2007) considered both critiques and pointed out that the 2001 DOJ policy
change (i.e., Ashcroft guidelines) directed the FBI and EOUSA to proactively disrupt terrorist
groups before they can act. In his study, Chesney used data from the American Terrorism Study
(ATS) and other sources to address the two criticisms raised above. First, he argued that it is
important to separate what most people universally agree to be terrorism cases (where defendants
are directly linked to terror groups and are engaging in acts of terrorism) from pretextual
prevention cases and diffused prevention cases. Pretextual prevention cases are those where the
government has some reason to suspect an individual is linked to a terrorist group, but there is no
evidence linking him/her to an act of terrorism (See Figure 2). In these cases the prosecutor
pursues any criminal charges that happen to be available. According to Chesney, diffused
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
15
prevention cases occur when the government lacks evidence linking any particular person to
particular terrorist threat, so the government engages in passive-defense and target-hardening
measures (Chesney, 2007, referencing Heymann, 1998). Diffused prevention cases involve
charges like immigration fraud and financial fraud. The argument is that terrorist groups
routinely engage in both types of behavior, so cracking down will interrupt terrorist planning.
15
Figure 2b
Case Type by Terrorism Link
Case Type
Linked to Extremist Group
or Ideology
Linked to a completed or
planned act of terrorism
Event-linked
Defendant(s) linked in
case documents
Defendants Linked in
Case Documents
Pretextual
Defendant(s) linked in
case documents
No Link
Diffusion*
No Link No Link
* some prosecutors have attempted to use circumstantial evidence to link a defendant to a
terrorist organization/ideology.
3. OUR APPROACH TO DEFINING TERRORISM
As mentioned above, neither academics nor politicians have agreed on a universal
definition of terrorism. Smith (1994) avoided the definition problem by using the FBI’s
“Terrorism Enterprise” investigation data. The Federal Bureau of Investigation defines terrorism
as: “[T]he unlawful use of force and violence against persons or property to intimidate or coerce
15
Like Chesney, TRAC categorizes cases as Terrorism and Anti-Terrorism (TRAC, 2003; 2006). The first category
contains cases that provide demonstrable links to a planned or complete terrorist act, and the second category
includes cases similar to Chesney’s diffused prevention and pretexual prosecution cases.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
16
a government, the civilian population, or any segment thereof, in furtherance of political or
social objectives” (Counterterrorism Threat Assessment and Warning Unit, 1999:5). Using the
FBI’s terrorism enterprise data provides a consistent measurement of the federal government’s
response to terrorism over time. That measurement has remained relatively unchanged for
almost three decades.
Authority for the FBI to open a terrorism enterprise investigation is provided in
guidelines promulgated by the U.S. Attorney General. For 25 years, the FBI compiled the names
of persons who were indicted in United States District Court after being the subject of such an
investigation. The FBI released those names to Brent Smith, who created the American
Terrorism Study database (ATS). The data for the database were gathered from public
documents for each of the names provided. The ATS includes information on almost every
person indicted as a result of a terrorism enterprise investigation from 1980 through 2004.
16
From 1980 until September 11, 2001, the FBI’s list provided one of the most consistent and
complete records of counterterrorism responses by the federal government.
Smith received names from the FBI in a series of lists. The early lists, containing names
from 1980 to 1998, contained each defendant’s name and the name of the group with which that
defendant was affiliated. That changed with the 1998-2002 list. For the first time and only in a
handful of cases filed after September 11, 2001, the list contained a new designation: no link to
terrorism. Subsequently, the 2002 to 2004 list did not contain group affiliation for any of the
defendants.
Another pattern emerged. Data collection revealed that the FBI routinely referred cases
to U.S. Attorneys for prosecution regardless of whether there was evidence linking a defendant
to an act of terrorism. Since 1980, the FBI had referred both event-linked cases and pretextual
16
A more detailed explanation of the ATS database is provided in Chapter 3.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
17
cases for prosecution. After September 11, however, the FBI began referring cases with no
clearly stated link to terrorism - a majority of those cases involved immigration and financial
fraud. The OIG (2007) reported some similar cases, but did not indicate how many it found
(OIG-07-04).
17i
4.
CONTEXT: U.S. ANTITERRORISM POLICY
Many experts believe that the events of September 11 were the result of an intelligence
failure (e.g.,Porch and Wirtz, 2002). Indeed, one of the key findings of the 9/11 Commission
Report focused on the lack of communication between two federal agencies with similar but
seemingly competing missions - the FBI and the CIA (National Commission on Terrorist Attacks
Upon the United States, 2004). To address that conclusion, Congress passed the USA PATRIOT
Act in October 2001. The Act expanded law enforcement authority to investigate suspected
terrorists, loosened restrictions on surveillance procedures, strengthened controls on international
money laundering, and authorized disclosure of foreign intelligence information obtained in
criminal investigation to intelligence and law enforcement agencies.
With the tools provided by Congress, America’s terrorism policy is interpreted and
implemented by the executive branch in four distinct chains of command. The first chain of
command begins with the President and runs through the National Security Council, which is
responsible for implementing international terrorism policy (Perl, 2003). The second chain of
17
These cases might be evidence a broader policy shift. While coding data, an ATS staff member found the affidavit
of an FBI field agent who stated that immigration cases had been assigned to the JTTF. The timing corresponds
with a policy shift inside both the FBI and DHS to coordinate investigations between the former and the Bureau of
Immigration & Customs Enforcement (the immigration investigative arm of DHS).
The timing also corresponds
to a dramatic increase in the number of JTTF field offices (34 to 66) and personnel assigned by the FBI to its
terrorism teams (600 to more than 7,000).
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
18
command runs from the President though the Director of the Department of Homeland Security
(DHS). The third chain of command runs from the President to the Secretary of State who heads
the U.S. State Department which contains the Office of the Coordinator for Counterterrorism.
The fourth chain runs from the President to the U.S. Attorney General who heads the U.S.
Department of Justice (DOJ). The FBI and the Executive Office of United States Attorneys
(EOUSA) are located within the DOJ. It is in the fourth chain of command that policies centered
on the investigation and prosecution of domestic terrorism are created and implemented.
Until the 1980s, the United States avoided practices that might result in terrorists being
viewed by the public as anything but common criminals. Responding to public consternation
over the Watergate scandal and COINTELPRO, Attorney General Edward Levi implemented
guidelines for the FBI that dramatically limited its authority to engage in domestic security
investigations (Levi Guidelines, 1976). Domestic security investigations dropped from 20,000
per year in 1973 to less than 300 in 1976 (Smith et al., 2002). When an investigation did occur,
terrorists were charged and prosecuted like traditional offenders (Smith and Damphousse, 1998).
In the early 1980s, a policy shift occurred in the wake of a string of armored-car
robberies perpetrated by leftist groups. Congress pressured Attorney General William F. Smith
and the FBI to implement new guidelines for terrorism investigations. In 1983, the Smith
Guidelines provided new authority for the FBI to investigate domestic terrorism groups (Smith
Guidelines, 1983). After opening an official terrorism investigation, the FBI could investigate
groups for longer periods of time than possible under a “general crimes” investigation. A
separate set of counterintelligence guidelines gave the FBI expanded authority to investigate
international terrorists.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
19
In the wake of September 11, Attorney General Ashcroft implemented new guidelines
that expanded the FBI’s authority to investigate domestic terrorism. The Smith Guidelines had
required FBI field offices to refer potential terrorism investigations, involving two or more
persons, to the Director or Assistant Director of the FBI; they, and only they, could authorize a
“terrorism enterprise” investigation. Once the Director authorized a terrorism investigation, he
had to report that fact to the Office of Intelligence Policy and Review. The Smith Guidelines
also required the director or another top official to monitor the progress of the investigation at
180-day intervals. Section (B)(4)(a) of the Ashcroft Guidelines loosened those standards by
allowing agents in the field to authorize a terrorism investigation for a period of up to one year.
18
The field office was required, within one year, to report to FBIHQ any terrorism investigations it
initiated and provide reports. Permission to open an investigation, however, was no longer
necessary. The new guidelines also centralized the analysis of fieldwork at FBI headquarters.
The Ashcroft guidelines remained in place through the end of this study.
It would be logical to assume that decentralizing the authority to open terrorism
investigations has changed what types of cases the FBI labels “terrorist.” That, coupled with
Attorney General Ashcroft’s directive to the FBI and the EOUSA to interrupt, arrest and
prosecute suspected terrorists before an act of terrorism can be committed should have
fundamentally altered the timing of when cases are formally entered into United States District
Courts. The proactive nature of the policy also suggests that the FBI may not be able to wait to
collect enough evidence to convict defendants of complex conspiracies, so the type and severity
of charges with which terrorist defendants have been indicted may have changed as well.
18
But unlike the Smith Guidelines, the Ashcroft Guidelines allow the Special Agent in Charge to renew the
investigation without interference from FBIHQ.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
20
5. THEORETICAL FOUNDATIONS
Given our previous discussion of terrorism and changes in the federal response to
terrorism over the past four decades, we now describe two theories that inform our research
questions. The first describes how social systems (such as the American justice system) become
loosely or more tightly “coupled” and the effect these changes have on how the system and its
actors function. The second theory similarly describes how changes in the justice system (such
as bail reform) can have dramatic effects on how discretion is used. These changes can affect
how both the prosecution and the defense behave in court proceedings.
5.a. Structural Contextual Theory. While conducting research on sentence disparity,
Hagan (1980) theorized that our ability to predict sentence outcomes is substantially better for
certain types of crimes than it is for others. With structural contextual theory, Hagan suggested
that the justice system was made up of “loosely coupled components” that work independently of
one another. Normally, those components compete for resources and occasionally pursue
different objectives. Hagan then suggested that some forms of crime catch the public’s attention.
When that happens, the public increases pressure on elected officials and individuals working in
criminal justice system to respond. Hagan argued that when political power is directed towards
particular forms of criminality, the system tightens in response through proactive techniques
(Hagan, 1989:118). When tightened coupling occurs, components of the criminal justice system
pool their resources and increase their focus on those forms of criminality. When that happens,
the disparity in sentence outcomes is reduced.
A number of studies have applied structural contextual theory to examine sentencing
outcomes. Using a structural contextual perspective, Smith and Damphousse (1998:88)
theorized that if terrorism was seen as a serious problem by members of the public, politicians,
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
21
and actors within the criminal justice system, one would expect increased levels of cooperation
between components of the criminal justice system. Cooperation, they agued, should limit
discretion as criminal justice actors investigate, prosecute and determine prison sentences after a
conviction. Therefore, the more seriously the public perceives terrorism, the more evidence of
“coupling” one should find. Smith and Damphousse found support for structural contextual
theory, reporting that nearly four times as much sentencing variance could be explained in
terrorism cases than non-terrorism cases. Additionally, they suggested that a proactive political
environment may be a more important predictor of sentence length than crime severity.
5.b. Trickle-up/Hydraulic Effect. After 9/11, Congress passed a number of measures
(discussed in more detail below) aimed at strengthening American counter- and antiterrorism
efforts and making punishment more severe for individuals who engage in terrorism. According
to Walker (1994), “get-tough” policy changes have a number of unintended consequences.
Walker theorized that, despite the increased focus, “get tough” measures would have no
measurable impact on the more serious crimes for which they were intended. Rather, he argued,
the majority of resources were already directed towards more serious crimes; thus, adding
resources would not increase the effectiveness of fighting more serous crimes.
Walker suggested that changes in policy directed towards serious offenses have the
greatest impact on lower-level offenses. According to the hydraulic effect, a “get tough” policy
shift raises less serious offenses to a more serious level by lowering the opportunities for
mitigation. In other words, a “get tough” policy change reduces a defendant’s opportunity to
plead to a lesser charge. Walker also argued that law enforcement personnel would engage in
net-widening. In an effort to appear more proactive and more effective, Walker argued, law
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
22
enforcement personnel and prosecutors investigate and charge defendants with crimes that would
have been ignored before. Because these policies reduce the chances for mitigation, the
sentences of lower-level defendants tend to rise above what those individuals would expect to
receive under normal circumstances.
Following this line of reasoning, the “war on terror” policy changes implemented after
9/11 should not have a dramatic effect on the more serious, targeted, crimes. Evidence of the
hydraulic effect should come in the form of defendants being charged with lower-level crimes,
and receiving longer sentences after a “get tough” policy is implemented. One would also expect
to find a higher frequency of low-level charges filed against a broader range of defendants after a
policy change. Damphousse and Shields (2007) tested the hydraulic effect and found some
support. Their analyses, however, were limited to testing a handful of cases filed between
September 11, 2001 and August 16, 2002.
6. RESEARCH QUESTIONS AND HYPOTHESES
Changes in how the FBI was allowed to investigate domestic security/ terrorism
enterprise cases brought about a change in the methods used by U.S. Attorneys to prosecute
terrorists. Until the 1980s, prosecutors followed the same philosophy that investigators used:
they treated terrorists like traditional offenders. In the 1960’s and 1970’s, a few prosecutors
experimented with politicizing a terrorism cases, but the practice was generally avoided (Smith
et al., 2002). Based on an analysis of political crime laws, Turk suggested that prosecutors used
one of two methods, explicit politicality or exceptional vagueness, to prosecute “political”
defendants (Turk, 2002). Explicit politicality was characterized by the government’s use of a
terrorist label in trial documents and the portrayal of the defendant as a terrorist to the public
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
23
through the news media. This strategy involved an extensive discussion of the defendants’
motives and the use of charges that include an element of conspiracy—alleging that the
defendant engaged in some type of politically motivated behavior. The most extreme example of
this type of charge would be seditious conspiracy or treason.
When the exceptional vagueness strategy was employed, the government avoided
describing the defendant as a terrorist. Instead, the defendant was depicted as a traditional
offender. Smith and Damphousse referred to this strategy as conventional criminality.
Subsequent research by Smith and Damphousse expanded Turk’s typology to include a third
prosecutorial strategy (Smith et al., 2002). After considering the challenges (i.e., acquittals) that
prosecutors faced in earlier, highly politicized terrorism cases, they pointed out that federal
prosecutors sometimes used a middle-ground approach, which they coined subtle innuendo. In
this strategy the government charged defendants under either a presumed liability statute or with
some “traditional” crime where motive was not an issue. At trial, the researchers found that
prosecutors “dropped” a series of subtle hints that the defendant was part of a terrorist group.
Later research by Damphousse and Shields (2007) found evidence that prosecutors sometimes
did more than drop subtle hints. On occasion defendants were labeled terrorists or directly
linked to groups known to be terrorist groups, but unlike explicit politicality cases, the
prosecution had no need to delve into political motive directly. For the purposes of this study,
the middle group will be called political innuendo.
The government’s foray into politicizing cases led defense attorneys to develop a number
of counter-strategies unique to terrorism cases. According to Smith and Damphousse (1998),
defense attorneys had success challenging politicized trials using due process claims. The most
successful strategy, however, involved defense attorneys who tried to distance their clients from
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
24
an alleged political ideology and/or group. A common technique used in disassociation
strategies involved filing motions to sever a defendant’s trial from a co-defendants’ trial. A
preliminary examination of data collected on 1990s terrorism cases revealed that 40 percent of
defendants who employed a disassociation strategy were either acquitted, received a mistrial, or
had all charges dismissed (Smith et al., 2005). The specific methods used to accomplish those
results varied from case to case, but it frequently involved using severances and motions in
limine to restrict prosecutors from using language that linked the defendant to a group or
ideological concept.
Other defendants have claimed that they were the target of political persecution. The
rationale for this strategy is to defeat the “motive” element alleged by the prosecutor by showing
that, instead of possessing a terrorist motive, the defendants’ beliefs were benign and they were
the target of an overzealous witch hunt because their beliefs fell outside of the mainstream. A
number of other defense methods have been tried with mostly unremarkable results.
19
Anecdotally, it appears as though the majority of defendants do not employ any “politicized”
defense strategy, their attorneys use strategies that are standard in other criminal cases (i.e.
traditional defense).
Research Question 1. What is the relationship between prosecutorial and defense
strategies, and do those strategies affect case outcomes?
Well over 90 percent of traditional federal criminal cases result in convictions (Bureau of
Justice, 2004). Prior research has found that acquittal rates are higher, and guilty plea rates are
19
Some defense attorneys have pursued a freedom fighter strategy, where the defendant claims that he or she was
part of a legitimate effort to replace an existing, but corrupt, government. Another defense strategy attempted to
portray the defendant’s actions as acts of
civil disobedience: Here, the defendants are portrayed not as criminals
(or terrorists) but as citizens who were exercising freedom of expression.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
25
lower, for terrorism cases than for traditional criminal cases. For that reason, terrorism cases that
are prosecuted like traditional criminal cases (e.g. conventional criminality) should result in
higher conviction rates than terrorism cases that are highly politicized. Because non-politicized
prosecution methods provide the defendant with little reason and/or incentive to use non-
traditional defenses, prosecution methods that center on conventional criminality will most likely
be met with a traditional defense strategy. The combination of the conventional criminality and
traditional defense will most resemble non-terrorism cases and should result in the highest
conviction rates.
Likewise, cases that are the explicitly politicized should result in the lowest conviction
rate. By definition, explicit politicality involves motive as an element of the case that must be
proved, opening the door to defense tactics and claims that are designed to raise reasonable
doubt. Because explicit politicality involves linking the defendant to a terrorist ideology,
disassociation should be the most successful defense strategy.
H
1
=A conventional criminality prosecution method and a traditional defense strategy
will produce the highest conviction rates.
H
2
= An explicit politicality prosecution method and the defense strategy,
disassociation, will produce the lowest conviction rates.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
26
Research Question 2. How has 9/11 impacted the ways the federal government responds to
terrorism?
ATS data, TRAC data, and DOJ reports all suggest that the number of terrorism cases
grew substantially after 9/11. According to structural contextual theory, an event like 9/11 will
heighten public awareness to the perceived threat of terrorism, causing actors within the criminal
justice system to work more closely together, in a proactive fashion, to allay public fears. This
“tightened coupling” should be measurable in terms of reduced discretion. Testing structural
contextual theory in terrorism cases, Smith and Damphousse (1998) found that plea bargain rates
were substantially lower in terrorism cases than non-terrorism cases (see also, Shields, et al,
2006, and, Damphousse and Shields, 2007). Bases on the application of structural contextual
theory in prior terrorism research, similar patterns should be found here.
H
3
=If tightened coupling has occurred, Prosecutors will voluntarily drop fewer counts
(charge bargaining) in the post-9/11 era.
H
4
= If tightened coupling has occurred, the percentage of plea bargained cases will
decrease among pretextual prosecutions in the post- 9/11 era.
The hydraulic effect suggests that “get tough” policies implemented in the wake of 9/11
will result in net widening. Net widening occurs in two different ways. First, offenders who
engaged in minor offenses that were ignored before the triggering event will receive new
attention from investigators and prosecutors. Second, because the level of serious criminal
activity is likely to remain relatively unchanged, investigators and prosecutors will seek to
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
27
prosecute offenders on new or novel charges. Damphousse and Shields (2007) found some
preliminary support for both of these propositions, but their analysis was limited to cases filed
prior to August 2002. If the hydraulic effect does occur, evidence of net-widening should be
present.
H
5
=If net widening has occurred, cases in the post-9/11 era will involve a higher
proportion of low-level offenses than cases filed before 9/11.
H
6
=If net widening has occurred, there will be higher proportion of pretextual
prosecutions as compared to event-linked prosecutions post-9/11.
Similarly, by intercepting cases sooner, the FBI is less likely to have time to develop
relationships with informants or infiltrate groups with government agents. As a result, there
should be a decrease in the amount of evidence available to put together large conspiracy cases.
H
7
=The number of defendants per case will decrease in the post-9/11 era.
H
8
= The proportion of event-linked cases involving confidential informants will be
smaller post-9/11.
Because prosecutors are bringing cases to trial sooner, they will be forced to file “kitchen sink”
indictments which contain counts alleging serious crimes, and counts that allege more traditional
and less serious charges.
H
9
= Conviction rates on more serious charges will decease in the post-9/11 era
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
28
III: DATA AND METHODS
This project used data from two sources: the American Terrorism Study and the
Prosecution and Defense Strategies database. Both databases are housed at the Terrorism
Research Center in Fulbright College at the University of Arkansas. The first database, the
American Terrorism Study (ATS), was created after the FBI released to Brent Smith the names
of persons indicted as a result of terrorism enterprise investigations from 1980 to 1989. The
Department of Justice matched the list of terrorist defendants with federal court docket numbers
assigned throughout the Unites States and Puerto Rico. Smith began collecting demographic and
sentencing data in each of the federal district courts where the trials occurred. Those data were
then supplemented by information provided by the Administrative Office of the U.S. Courts.
20
That first list of names was supplemented by four additional lists. The ATS includes the
names of persons who have been indicted as the result of a FBI terrorism investigation (under the
Attorney General’s Guidelines) from 1980 through 2004. The ATS database contains
information on 706 indictees charged with 9,633 criminal violations in 254 court cases. In
addition, there are data on approximately 75 different terrorist groups.
The ATS dataset is comprised of approximately 80 variables that measure defendant
demographic information (e.g., race, sex, age, income, education level, and marital
status),general case information (e.g., number of counts, year of indictment, criminal statute,
length of case, case outcome, and sentence length), terrorism specific information (e.g., type of
terrorism, group affiliation, length of membership, role in group, how recruited, intended
targets, and actual targets), and some case information unique to terrorism (e.g., prosecution
20
After receiving the FBI list, data collection teams visited the federal courthouses and archive facilities to review
the identified cases and copy documents. Data gleaned from those documents were then coded into approximately
80 variables and entered into the American Terrorism Study database. (Smith and Damphousse, 2004).
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
29
methods, defense methods, and amount of community sympathy). While the coding of many of
the variables is straightforward (e.g., age, gender, sentence length), the coding of other latent
variables (e.g., prosecution and defense strategy) requires more attention. These variables are
coded through a vetting process conducted by the research team. After the case material has
been carefully inspected, the team determines by consensus how each of the latent variables
should be coded.
1. ATS DATA DESCRIPTION
This project uses several of the ATS demographic variables. Gender is a nominal level
variable. For this study male is coded 1, and female is coded 0. The database population is 89.1
percent male. Race is a nominal level variable, and it is coded into 6 categories in the ATS. For
the purposes of this study, race was recoded into three categories: Caucasian, Black, and
Hispanic/Other. Caucasians make up 69.2
Table 3.01
Frequency Distribution: ATS Race
Race Frequency Percent
Caucasian
443 69.2
Black
66 10.3
Hispanic /Other
98 15.4
Total
607 94.8
Missing System
33 5.2
Total
640 100.0
percent of the population. Unfortunately, Middle Eastern defendants of Arabic descent are
included as Caucasian (N = 129). For regression models the variable was coded into dummy
variables. One dummy variable was created for Caucasian defendants and one was created for
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
30
black defendants. Age is a ratio level variable and coded in years of age at the date of indicted.
The mean age for the population is 37.8 years.
Year of Indictment is an ordinal level variable and measured at the time the indictment
was filed. For analyses comparing pre-9/11 defendants to post-9/11 defendants, individuals
indicted in 2001 were eliminated (n=27). Of the remaining indictees (N=679), those who were
indicted in 2000 or before were coded 0, whereas indictees indicted in cases filed in 2002 or after
were coded 1. Table 3.02 presents the size of the pre- and post-9/11 samples. Total counts is an
ordinal level variable
Table 3.02
Size of Pre-and Post-9/11 Samples
Era Frequency Percent
Pre 9/11
512 72.5
Post-9/11
167 23.7
Total
679 96.2
Missing System
27 3.8
Total
706 100.0
and measures the total number of counts filed against each defendant. The range for the data set
is 1 count to 652 counts. Over 50 percent of the defendants in the database were charged with 3
counts or less, and 90 percent of the defendants were charged with 16 counts or fewer. There are
only 4 court cases in the database where defendants were charged with 60 or more counts.
Group Type is a nominal level variable, coded into 6 categories: domestic right-wing groups (1),
domestic left-wing groups (2), international groups (3), environmental groups (4), single issue
(5) and lone wolf (6). Individuals not linked to a group in the FBI list, or in case documents,
were coded system missing unless
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
31
Table 3.03
Frequency Distribution: Group Type
Group Type Frequency Percent
Rightwing
115 18.0
Leftwing
201 31.4
International
210 32.8
Environmental
14 2.2
Single Issue
6 .9
Lone Wolf
4 .6
Total
550 85.9
Missing System
90 14.1
Total
640 100.0
internet searches of media documents revealed a link. Table 3.03 contains the frequencies, and
indicates that 86 percent of the defendants in the database are linked to a group or ideology.
Role is a nominal level variable and is coded in the ATS database to capture the varied and
unique roles that individuals hold in their organizations (e.g. leader, recruiter, bomb maker,
subordinate, Holy man). For this study, role was recoded into a dummy variable. Persons
identified as having a leadership role were coded 1 and all others were coded 0.
Target is a dummy variable that will be created out of actual primary target and intended
target. Actual primary target is a nominal level variable (categorical) and it indicates the first
target the group was charged with successfully attacking. Intended Target is a nominal level
variable (Categorical) and indicates the first target the group intended to attack but did not hit.
If case documents indicate that the defendant either intended or actually hit a target, the variable
target will be coded 1, and all other cases will be coded 0. 48 percent (N=344) of the defendants
in the sample were indicted after attacking a target of some kind, another 21 percent (N= 147)
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
32
were indicted before they could attack their intended targets. As Table 3.04 shows, 30 percent of
the defendants in the sample were not linked to an actual or intended target.
Table 3.04
Frequency Distribution: Defendant Linked to a Target
Link or not Frequency Percent
Linked to target
491 69.5
No target
215 30.5
Total
706 100.0
Overall case outcome (case_res)is a nominal level variable. In the database, this variable
is coded into more than 20 possible outcomes. In order to use case result in bivariate analyses
with prosecutorial method (pros_meth), defense method (def_meth), and case type (case_type), it
was recoded to have three values. The frequencies are found in table 3.05. There were 87 cases
in the system missing category. The majority
Table 3.05
Frequency distribution: Case Outcomes
Frequency Percent
Trial conviction
182 25.8
Pleaded guilty
336 47.6
Dismiss or acquit
101 14.3
Total
619 87.7
Missing System
87 12.3
Total
706 100.0
of the missing cases are defendants who were indicted but remained fugitive at the time data
were collected. Some cases involve defendants who were indicted in more than one federal
district court and were subsequently transferred. The case outcome variable was also recoded
into two dummy variables. The first dummy variable, conviction, coded cases that resulted in
any type of conviction as 1, and cases which resulted in acquittal, mistrial or dismissal as zero.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
33
The second dummy variable, plead_guilty, was coded to include cases where a conviction of any
kind resulted in the absence of a trial. The final outcome dummy variable, trial_conv, includes
only cases where the defendant was convicted at trial (both bench trials and jury trials).
This study also uses a measurement of count severity (ct_sev). An interval level variable,
it measures the severity of each count with which a defendant is charged. Count severity is
coded on a scale of increasing severity from 1 to 29 (see appendix 1). To measure sentence
length, this study uses a ratio level variable (sent_mon) measuring the total time (months) that a
defendant is sentenced to prison. The sentence variable includes life sentences and the death
penalty. To give those sentences a numerical value, a frequency distribution was performed.
The longest sentence in months was 2,880 months. Life sentences were recoded to 2,881
months, and the death penalty was recoded to 2,882 months.
Sever is a nominal, dichotomous variable and indicates that a defendant has employed a
disassociation defense strategy. A defendant who filed a motion to have his or her case tried
separately from another defendant in the case was coded 1. If no severance motion was filed, the
variable was coded 0. About 22 percent of the defendants in the sample attempted to sever their
cases. Verbiage is a nominal, dichotomous variable. The variable was coded 1 if a defendant
filed a motion in limine to block the prosecution from using words or references that might link
the defendant to a known terrorist, a terrorist group, a particular ideology/religious belief or a
particular terrorist act. About 28 percent of the defendants in the sample attempted to limit
verbiage. Sever_ct is a nominal, dichotomous variable that measures whether the defendant
attempted to sever a count from the indictment, as some defendants attempt to remove counts
that include references to terrorism. If a defendant filed a motion to sever a count, the variable
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
34
was coded 1, and if not, the variable was coded 0. Number of Motions is a ratio level variable
indicating the number of motions filed by each defendant (median = 11).
The ATS variables listed above provide basic demographic information for this study. In
addition, data from the ATS permitted analyses of case outcomes, sentence lengths, and count
severity. Unfortunately, ATS data allow only a limited inquiry into the legal strategies used in
federal terrorism trials. For example, the ATS tracks the total number of motions filed by
defendants during a trial (motions), but it does not differentiate between the types of motions
filed nor does it track their outcomes. Similarly, the ATS contains a dichotomous variable that
measures if any defendants in a case turn state’s evidence (i.e. testified on behalf of the
prosecution). That variable (prosevid) does not indicate what kind of testimony/evidence the
defendant provided, and it does not indicate which defendant testified. In effect, one can only
determine whether a defendant turned state’s evidence. It is impossible to identify the defendant,
and therefore, it is impossible to measure whether the defendant received any benefit from doing
so. These are only a few of the ATS limitations. Thus, this project led to the creation the
Prosecution and Defense Strategies database.
2. PROSECUTION AND DEFENSE STRATEGY (PADS) VARIABLES
For this project, the ATS was supplemented with data from the Prosecution and Defense
Strategies project (PADS). The PADS project built a database from cases that had been
collected as part of the ATS database. Because the PADS database uses the same cases at the
ATS, the demographic information is the same. Over 150 additional variables that measure trial
specific data were created and coded (thereby fulfilling our fourth objective). The PADS
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
35
database contains variables that measure the strategies used by legal counsel as well as other
legal nuances.
For example, PADS variables track information about the type of attorney used. The
database also includes variables that measure whether the defendant received bail, and if not, the
reason bail was denied. One set of variables track whether a superseding indictment was filed in
each case, and another set of variables track the number of counts added or dropped from the
original indictment. These counts are coded by statute number and by United States Code
Chapter. These data also track defense motions and their outcomes, for example: defense
challenges to FISA; motions to suppress physical evidence; motions to suppress electronic
surveillance; motions to sever counts; motions to suppress statements, and; an entire range of pro
se motions. Similarly, PADS Data track prosecution motions and outcomes (e.g., whether CIPA
protection was sought, motions to exclude defense evidence, and challenges to defense
strategies).
3. PADS DATA DESCRIPTION
This project used a number of variables that measured legal strategies. The variable
prosmeth exists in the ATS database and was recoded for PADS.
21
Prosecutorial Method
(prosmeth_recode) is an ordinal level variable. This variable is coded into three categories (see
table 3.06). The first category is conventional criminality (coded 1) and involves cases in which
the defendant has been charged with conventional criminal charges (i.e. no motive element) and
the prosecution makes no attempt to link the defendant to a terrorist organization or a terrorist
act. The next category, political innuendo, (coded 2) is composed of cases in which a defendant
21
The ATS variable prosmeth contained four categories: the three discussed here, and a fourth, material support,
that was coded system missing for this study.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
36
has been indicted on conventional criminal charges and in which the prosecution has linked the
defendant, expressly or impliedly, to a terrorist group or act of terrorism. The final category is
explicit politicality (coded 3)and is comprised of cases involving counts that draw into question
the defendant’s motive for committing a crime (e.g. sedition, conspiracy to murder, etc.), and
cases where the defendant is publicly linked to a terrorist group.
Table 3.06
Frequency Distribution: prosecution methods used
Prosecution Methods Frequency Percent
Conventional criminality
149 21.1
Political innuendo
170 24.1
Explicit politicality
385 54.5
Total
704 99.7
Missing System
2 .3
Total
706 100.0
Similar to prosecution strategy variables, this study uses defense strategy variables
(defmeth) from the ATS and recoded them into a new variable.
22
The Defense strategy variable
(defmeth_recode) is a nominal level variable. For bivariate analyses, the three most common
defense methods from the ATS were used. The first defense strategy, political persecution,
(coded 1) consists of those cases were the defendant claims that he or she is innocent and being
prosecuted because of his or her political and/or religious beliefs. Second, the defense strategy
disassociation (coded 2) is comprised of cases where the defendant attempted to distance
herself/himself from group members and/or an ideology. Finally, traditional (coded 3) consists
of cases where the defense used a traditional criminal defense. The remaining defense strategies
22
The three most common defense strategies were retained, as explained here. The remaining defmeth categories
(civil disobedience, freedom fighter, jurisdiction challenge, etc) were coded “system missing” because of their
low frequency (N=61).
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
37
were coded system missing (n =89). Table 3.07 provides the frequency distribution for the
defmeth_recode.
Table 3.07
Frequency Distribution: defense methods used
Defense Methods Frequency Percent
Political persecution
120 17.0
Disassociation
179 25.4
Traditional
318 45.0
Total
617 87.4
Missing System
89 12.6
Total
706 100.0
Link Made is a nominal level variable with 4 categories (linked to group; not linked non-
immigration; not linked immigration, and; associated with known member) in the PADS
database. For this study, it was recoded into a dummy variable with linked coded 1, and all
others coded 0. For cases that were identified on the FBI lists from 1980 to 2002, coding was
based exclusively on the FBI’s group designation information to determine whether the
defendant was linked to a group. For post-9/11 cases, defendants were coded as linked to
terrorist groups if a reference to such an affiliation was made in the case documents or after if an
internet search of media stories provided a link. This means that the post-9/11 group may be
more under-inclusive than the pre- 9/11 group, but that is the best alternative currently available.
Bail is a nominal level variable measuring whether or not a defendant was released on bail. If
the defendant was released, the variable was coded 1. If the defendant remained incarcerated,
the variable was coded 0. Research by Albonetti (1990) found that the reason bail is denied can
impact the severity of sentence a defendant receives. Namely, when a defendant is deemed
dangerous, and held without bail, Albonetti found that he or she is more likely to receive a longer
sentence. The variable baildeny was coded to measure the reasons why a defendant was held
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
38
without bail. Defendants remanded to custody because of an immigration hold were coded 1.
Defendants remanded to custody because they posed a flight risk were coded 2. Defendants who
were found to pose a risk to the community were coded 3. Defendants released on bail were
coded 0. The frequency distribution for bail is presented in Table 3.08. Of those defendants for
which we had information, 66 percent were denied bail. The frequency distribution for the
reason bail was denied is found in table 3.09. In 73 cases no reason could be determined for why
bail was denied. Of the remaining cases, exactly half were retained because they were deemed
dangerous.
Table 3.08
Frequency Distribution: Was Bail Granted
Bail Granted Frequency Percent
Bail denied
427 60.5
Bail granted
212 30.0
Total
639 90.5
Missing System
67 9.5
Total
706 100.0
The PADS database contains the variable attorney_type; it is a nominal level variable
categorizing the type of counsel employed by each defendant. Information on attorney type was
available in 79 percent of the sample (N = 557). Attorneys appointed under the Criminal Justice
Act to represent indigent defendants were coded 0, federal public defenders were coded 1,
private attorneys were coded 2, and defendants who act pro se were coded 3. The results of the
frequency distribution for attorney type are presented in table 3.10.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
39
Table 3.09
Frequency distribution: reason bail was denied.
Reason Bail Denied Frequency Percent
Held by another jurisdiction or BICE/INS
105 14.9
Flight
72 10.2
Danger
44 6.2
Flight and danger
133 18.8
Total
354 50.1
Missing System
352 49.9
Total
706 100.0
Table 3.10
Frequency distribution: attorney type
Attorney type Frequency Percent
CJA appointment
174 24.6
Federal public defender
164 23.2
Private attorney
160 22.7
Pro se
59 8.4
Total
557 78.9
Missing System
149 21.1
Total
706 100.0
Many studies have found that witnesses affect whether a case goes to trial or results in a
plea bargain. For example, LaFree (1980) found that plea bargain rates were affected by
evidentiary variables: as the number of witnesses available to the prosecutor increases, so does
the probability of a guilty plea. LaFree’s findings are consistent with other studies (e.g. Harris
and Springer, 1989; Emmelman, 1996). Albonetti (1990), however, found that an increase in the
number of eyewitnesses exerted a negative effect on pleading guilty. The PADS database
includes variables measuring three different types of witnesses common in many terrorism cases:
the defendant who turns state’s evidence, the un-indicted group member who works as a
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
40
confidential informant; and, the federal law enforcement officer who infiltrates the group as
undercover agent.
To analyze the impact of a defendant turning state’s evidence, the variable def_test was
used. Def_test is a dichotomous variable. The variable was coded 1 if a defendant testified (or
agreed to testify) against his co-defendants at trial. All other instances were coded 0. The
confidential informant variable (info_mem) is a dichotomous variable. It measures whether a
confidential informant (not indicted, and non-law enforcement) provided the prosecution with
any information, evidence, or sworn testimony in the case (1 = yes, 0 = no). In almost 40 percent
of the cases studied the prosecution had a confidential informant in place.
Similarly, the government agent variable (infil_gov) is a dichotomous variable that
indicates whether the prosecution had information from a law enforcement official who
infiltrated the terrorist group. The government successfully infiltrated groups in 17 percent of
the cases in the sample. The latter two variables also include ordinal level variables (government
agents = infil_assist; confidential informants = infor_assist) that indicate what kind of
information was provided. Both variables were coded in ascending order: information only =1;
recordings =2; sworn testimony = 3; and, recordings & sworn testimony = 4. While the
information was not available for every case, the level of assistance provided by confidential
informants is displayed in table 3.11. Likewise, the level of assistance provided by government
agents who infiltrated groups is provided in table 3.12.
Like the existence of witnesses, the strength of physical evidence available to a
prosecutor is also an important factor that shapes how cases are resolved. Champion (1989)
found that 34 percent of prosecutors would intensify punitive severity when they believed they
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
41
Table 3.11
Level of assistance provided by confidential informants
Informant Assistance Level Frequency Percent
Information only
28 11.0
Recordings
16 6.3
Sworn testimony
23 9.1
Recordings and sworn
testimony
17 6.7
Unknown assistance
17 6.7
Total
101 39.9
N/A*
153 60.1
Total
254 100.0
* court records did not reveal the involvement of a confidential informant
Table 3.12
Level of assistance provided by undercover government agents
Agent Assistance Level Frequency Percent
Information only
4 1.6
Recordings
2 .8
Sworn testimony
5 2.0
Recordings and sworn
testimony
31 12.2
Unknown Assistance
11 .5
Total
53 17.0
N/A*
211 83.0
Total
254 100.0
* court records did not reveal the involvement of an undercover agent
had strong evidence. Similarly, 82 percent of prosecutors would decrease punitive severity in
plea bargains if they believed the evidence against the defendant was weak, (Champion, 1989).
La Free (1980) found that as the amount of evidence available to a prosecutor increased so did
the probability of a guilty plea (see also, Albonetti, 1990). This study uses several PADS
variables that measure evidentiary strength.
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
42
One imperfect proxy measure of evidentiary strength is its ability to survive a
suppression hearing. This study analyzes a variable that measures attempts to suppress evidence
(sup_evid). The suppression variable is a nominal level variable measuring whether a defendant
filed a motion to suppress physical evidence and the grounds for the motion. For example, if the
grounds for suppression are illegal searches and seizures, the variable was coded 1. If the
grounds for suppression are violations of the Federal Rules of Evidence (F.R.E.) 403/404where
the focus is on whether the potential prejudice to which the evidence exposes the defendant is
outweighed by probative value of the evidence, then the variable was coded 2. Likewise,
sup_stmt is a nominal level variable measuring whether a defendant filed a motion to suppress
his or her own statement (1), a co-defendant’s statement (2), or a witnesses statement (3).
Most often, data on motions to suppress were found on the case dockets. Some older
cases were missing dockets, and those were coded system missing for both variables. Only in
cases with dockets or an otherwise complete case record, were either of the suppression variables
coded. Table 3.13 provides the frequency distribution for motions to suppress evidence. Over
half of the defendants in the sample (for which we had information) filed a motion to suppress.
Table 3.13
Frequency distribution: motion to suppress evidence
Motion to Suppress Evidence Frequency Percent
None filed
268 38.0
Motion filed - search and seizure
violations
305 43.2
Motion filed - 403 challenge (prejudicial)
16 2.3
Total
573 81.2
Missing System
133 18.8
Total
706 100.0
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
43
Those motions were denied in 76 percent of the cases, and granted in 6 percent. 18 percent of
the suppression motions were pending when the case was settled. Perhaps settling the case was
hurried in an attempt to avoid having the evidence thrown out.
Table 3.14 displays the frequency distribution of motions to suppress or exclude
statements. Defendants filed these motions in 37 percent of the cases in which we had
information. As the table shows, the majority of those motions dealt with the defendants own
statements. About 71 percent of the motions to suppress statements were denied, and 7 percent
were granted. As with motions to suppress evidence, 10.5 percent of the motions to suppress
statements were pending when the case was settled. Both of these findings will be explored in
Chapter 4.
Table 3.14
Frequency Distribution: motion to suppress statements
Motion Suppress Statements Frequency Percent
None filed
367 52.0
Motion in limine to supress own statements
99 14.0
Motion in limine to exclude co-defendant
statements
8 1.1
Motion to exclude witness statements
28 4.0
Combination 1 and 3
78 11.0
Combination 1,2 & 3
3 .4
Motion to exclude expert witness
statements
3 .4
Total
586 83.0
Missing System
120 17.0
Total
706 100.0
4. CASE TYPE
Chesney (2007) provides the basic framework that this project will use to categorize
cases. Categorization will bypass the confusion concerning whether a case should be labeled
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
44
“terrorist” by separating defendants who are linked to a terrorist group or ideology from those
who are not. Even though Chesney’s study was limited to cases involving material support
charges, he recognized that post-9/11 antiterrorism policy directed the FBI and the EOUSA to
intercept and disrupt terrorist planning before an act of terrorism could occur. According to
Chesney, the FBI and EOUSA targeted immigration and financial fraud in a strategy unique to
this policy shift. These cases involved defendants who were neither linked to a terrorist
group/ideology nor an act of terrorism; these cases are at the center of the over-estimation
controversy.
Chesney refers to them as diffusion cases. They are “diffused” in that the DOJ identified
financial fraud and identification/immigration fraud as types of crime that international terrorist
groups must engage in order to plan and carry out attacks on American soil. Making it more
difficult for anyone to engage in these types of criminality, the argument goes, will disrupt a
necessary avenue of planning and preparation used by terrorists. Anecdotally, we believe that
cases of this nature were included in the post-9/11 list of names that the FBI provided to the
ATS. If those cases can be identified, they can be separated from cases that have historically
been “accepted” as terrorism cases allowing a more accurate measure of pre- and post-9/11 case
differences.
While Chesney identifies some general characteristics of the three different types of
cases, he made no attempt to analyze all three. This study begins by establishing distinct
parameters for each category. The first category, event-linked, includes defendants linked to a
terrorist group (or a terrorist philosophy as in the case of lone wolves) and linked to a planned or
completed act of terrorism. A second category of terrorism cases is composed of what Chesney
calls “pretextual” prosecutions. Those are cases in which a defendant is linked to a terrorist
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
45
group/ideology but not charged with a crime linked to an act of terrorism. As noted above, the
FBI has reported both types of cases prior to 9/11. Finally, the third category includes
“diffusion” prosecutions. In these cases the defendant is neither linked to a terrorist
group/ideology nor to an act of terrorism.
The variable case_type was created using the following coding scheme: Event-linked is
comprised of defendants who are linked to terrorists groups (link_made = 1) and who were
indicted on charges related to an intended/completed act of terrorism. To achieve the second
qualification, the ATS variables for intended target (int1targ) and actual target (act1targ) were
recoded into one dichotomous variable (target_recode). If there were actual or intended targets
identified in the case documents, the target_recode variable was coded 1. If there were no
targets, it was coded 0. The second category, pretextual, includes defendants who are linked to a
terrorism group (link_made = 1), but not linked in the case documents to a particular act of terror
(target_recode = 0). The third category, diffusion, is comprised of defendants who are not
linked to a group (link_made = 0) nor an act of terrorism (target_recode = 0).
Table 3.15
Frequency Distribution: case type
Case Type Frequency Percent
Valid Event linked
491 69.5
Pretextual
129 18.3
Diffusion
86 12.2
Total
706 100.0
Table 3.16 provides the frequency distribution for case type within the data set.
Approximately 70 percent of defendants in the study were linked to a group or ideology and
some type of target. 13 percent of the defendants in the study were linked to a group or
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
46
ideology, but fell into the pretextual category. Just over 12 percent of the defendants in the
sample fell into the diffusion category. As expected, all of the diffusion cases were filed after
9/11. Appendix7 contains the frequency distribution for the types of crimes with which
defendants were charged in diffusion cases. Nearly 85 percent of all the counts filed in diffusion
cases involved some form of financial or identity fraud.
Conversely, only 1.3 percent of cases involve firearms, explosives, or hazardous materials
charges. These findings support Chesney’s observations (2007) as mentioned in Chapter 2.
Analyses presented in Chapter 4 on the pre- and post-9/11 samples were performed using this
coding scheme.
Both the ATS and PADS datasets are constructed in flat files, and SPSS version 15.0 was
used to perform the analyses. The basic unit of organization for both datasets is the criminal
count. That means that data for each individual are coded for each count in which he or she has
been indicted. The following pages refer to different levels of analysis (e.g., count level, person
level, case level). The different levels of analysis were conducted by performing “select if”
functions to narrow them. Rather than repeat those steps for each hypothesis, Table 3.16
provides a coding key.
Table 3.16
Select if coding table
Analysis level Select if functions performed Output
Person level
ct_num=1 & ind_num =1
One line of data per person on 1
s
t
count in 1
s
t
indictment. Only one entry regardless of the
number of indictments.
Indictee level
ct_num = 1
One line of data on 1
s
t
count in each case that a
person has been indicted.
Group level
grp_sel = 1
One line of data on 1
s
t
count in 1
s
t
indictment for
each group. Only one entry regardless of the
number of indictments filed against group.
Case level
Case =1
One line of data per case on 1
s
t
count in 1
s
t
indictment filed against the first person in each
case.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
47
5. Research Question One
The following section describes the methods we used to determine what the most
significant prosecutorial and defense strategies have been over the past two decades. This
section also describes the methods used to determine whether any significant relationships exist
between prosecution methods, defense methods and overall conviction rates.
H
1
=A conventional criminality prosecution method and a traditional defense strategy
will produce the highest conviction rates.
H
2
= An explicit politicality prosecution method and the defense
strategy, disassociation, will produce the lowest conviction rates.
To test the first two hypotheses, we limited the database to the indictee level analysis.
We then ran two sets of crosstabs between the defense method (defmeth_recode) and prosecution
method (prosmeth_recode) by the dichotomous variable conviction (0 = no conviction, 1 =
conviction). The chi-square indicated that the conviction rates for both models were random (p <
.001 for each). The results of both models were combined in one table to provide frequencies
and an estimate of conviction rates. The results will be discussed in detail in Chapter 4, but they
suggest that prosecutors who employed an explicit politicality strategy suffered a penalty in net
conviction rates. The results also suggested the same for defendants who employed the political
persecution defense strategy.
Because conviction is a dichotomous variable, and because we wanted to predict the
effect of the different prosecution and defense strategies on likelihood of conviction, we used
binary logistic regression. We entered conviction (yes = 1, no = 0) as the dependant variable.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
48
The variable race (0 = non-white, 1 = white) and gender (0 = female, 1= male) were entered. As
a surrogate measure of evidentiary strength, we included the variable for whether a government
undercover agent was used (infil_gov) and the dichotomous variable for whether a confidential
informant was used (inform_mem). We included conspiracy (conspiracy charge used =1, not
used = 0) to provide a surrogate measure of case complexity, as conspiracy cases are typically
considered more complex than traditional cases. We also entered the ordinal variable count_sev
(see appendix 1 for coding) as a measure of the seriousness of the charges.
To test strategies, we included the dummy variables for political persecution defense
strategy and disassociation defense (leaving traditional defense out of the model to serve as a
reference category). Finally, we included the dichotomous variables for explicit politicality
prosecution strategy and political innuendo prosecution strategy (We left out conventional
criminality to serve as the reference category). The model was run as second time including
traditional defense in the analysis and leaving disassociation out as a reference. We also
substituted conventional criminality prosecution method with political innuendo. The results are
presented in Chapter 4.
6. Research Question Two
The following section describes the methods used to determine what impact 9/11 had on
the way the government responds to terrorism cases. The results generally support hypothesis
based on structural contextual theory and the hydraulic effect. However, an exception emerged
in the form of lower plea bargain rates in the post-9-/11 era.
H
3
=If tightened coupling has occurred, prosecutors will voluntarily drop fewer counts
(charge bargaining) in the post-9/11 era.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
49
H
4
= If tightened coupling has occurred, the percentage of plea bargained cases will
decrease among pretextual prosecutions in the post- 9/11 era.
H
5
=If net widening has occurred, cases in the post-9/11 era will involve a higher
proportion of low-level offenses than cases filed before 9/11.
H
6
=If net widening has occurred, there will be higher proportion of pretextual
prosecutions as compared to event-linked prosecutions in the post-9/11 era.
H
7
=The number of defendants per case will decrease among event-linked cases in the
post-9/11 era.
H
8
= The proportion of event-linked cases involving informants will be smaller in the
post-9/11 era.
H
9
= Conviction rates on more serious charges will decease in the post-9/11 era
We tested Hypothesis 3 at the indictee level. Because dismissals can be measured in two
ways, two analyses were performed. The variable date was recoded into a dichotomous variable.
Cases indicted prior to January 1, 2001 were coded 0 and cases filed after December 31, 2001
were coded 1. In the first analysis, the variable case_outcome was coded into a dichotomous
variable with dismissed =1, and all other values = 0. We then ran a bivariate analysis comparing
means for the two groups. For the second analysis we used cts_drop. It is an ordinal level
measuring the total number of counts dropped for each defendant. A bivariate analysis
compared the mean number of counts dropped for both groups. Hypothesis 4 was tested using
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
50
the same sample. A select if was performed further limiting the data base to only pretextual
cases in both pre- and post-9/11 groups. The variable for case outcome was recoded into a
dummy in order to isolate plea bargains. If a case ended as a result of a plea bargain, it was
coded 1. All other outcomes were coded 0. We then ran a bivariate analysis comparing the
proportion of plea bargained cases in each group.
To test hypothesis 5, we ran the analysis at the indictee level using the pre- and post-9/11
dummy variable as the predictor. To measure the level of offense, we used the variable for count
severity (ct_sev). Then we ran a bivariate analysis comparing the means of both groups. To
control for the possible impact of diffusion cases in the post-9/11 sample, we ran the analysis a
second time with diffusion cases removed. We tested Hypothesis 6 with two analyses. We ran a
crosstabs in the first analysis at the indictee level. We used the pre-9/11 and post-9/11 dummy
variable as the predictor and case type as the dependent. For the next analysis, a select if was
performed to limit the data base to the case level. Then we ran a crosstabs between case type and
the 9/11 dummy variable.
For hypothesis 7, we used case level analysis. After performing a “select if” to limit the
sample to event-linked cases (group_type =1), we used the same 9/11 dummy variable as the
previous hypotheses. We used the variable defend#, and ran a bivariate analysis comparing the
means of the pre- and post-9/11 samples. To test hypothesis 8, we again limited the database to
event-linked cases. To compare the means of the pre- and post-9/11 samples, we used the
dichotomous variable gov_infor and ran it as the dependant variable with the 9/11 dummy
variable as the predictor. Next, we ran the same model using gov_infil to determine the average
number of undercover agents available per case before and after 9/11.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
51
Because some cases involve multiple confidential informants and undercover agents, and
logic suggests that as the number of witnesses who are available to testify goes up, so do the
chances of a successful conviction, we ran both models again using a second set of variables. In
the third model we analyzed the variable infor_num to compute the average number of
confidential informants. Before we ran the model, we performed a select if to limit the database
so that only cases in which a confidential informant was used. In the forth model we analyzed
the variable infil_num to determine the average number of undercover agents. Like we did
above, the database was limited to cases were an undercover agent was used.
Finally, because not all confidential informants and undercover agents provide the same
amount and type of information, we ran two additional models. In the next model, we used the
ordinal level variable infor_level. As a reminder, infor_level is coded as follows: 1 =
information only; 2 = recordings; 3 = sworn testimony, and; 4 = all the above. For interpretation
purposes, those values were labeled as follows: 1 = low; 2 = low-moderate; 3 = high-moderate,
and; 4 = high. In the final model, we ran the analysis again using infil_level. The variable
infil_level is coded using the same scheme as infor_level.
To test hypothesis 9, we ran two logistic regression models. In the first, we limited the
cases to pre-9/11 by running a select if function in SPSS. We used convict (yes = 1, no = 0) as
the dependant variable and selected count severity, an interval variable ranging increasing
severity from 0 to 28, as the predictor. In the second model, we limited the cases to post-9/11 by
performing a select if function in SPSS. We used convict as the dependant variable and selected
count severity as the predictor.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
52
IV: FINDINGS FORTWO RESEARCH QUESTIONS
1. RESEARCH QUESTION ONE
This section contains the results from analyses conducted to determine whether a
relationship between prosecutorial and defense strategies exists, and if so, what effect those
strategies might have on case outcomes. The results generally support the hypotheses.
Hypothesis 1 predicted that a conventional criminality prosecutorial strategy and a traditional
defense would produce the highest conviction rate.
The conviction rates for the nine possible combinations of prosecutorial and defense
strategies were calculated. Those results are provided in Table 4.01, and indicate that the
conventional criminality prosecution strategy and the traditional defense strategy was the most
common combination in the model, but the combination did not produce the highest conviction
rate. Conventional criminality and a traditional defense were used in 127 cases with a 92.1
percent conviction rate—well above the overall conviction rate of 84.1 percent. Independent of
any defense strategy, conventional criminality produced the highest conviction rates among all
prosecution strategies (92.5 percent). However, 100 percent conviction rates occurred when
conventional criminality was combined with either disassociation (N=2) or political persecution
(N=5).
There was another prosecution /defense strategy combination that produced higher
conviction rates than the hypothesis predicted. The political innuendo prosecution strategy and
the political persecution defense produced a conviction rate of 96.7 percent (n=30). As an
overall prosecution strategy, political innuendo was slightly less successful than conventional
criminality, producing a conviction rate of 88.9 percent compared to 92.5 percent for the latter.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
53
The only other significant combination that produced results higher than the overall model
conviction rate was the political innuendo prosecution strategy combined with a traditional
defense. That combination resulted in convictions in 88.2 percent of the cases (N=51).
Table 4.01
Frequency Table of Prosecution Strategy and Defense Strategy conviction rates
23
Prosecution Strategy
Overall
conviction
rate by def
method
Conventional
Criminality
Political
Innuendo
Explicit
Politicality
Defense
Strategy
Political
Persecution
5/5*
100% (conv rate)
29/30
96.7 %
57/68
83.8%
91/103
88.3%
Disassociation
2/2
100 %
31/37
83.7%
97/122
79.5%
130/161
80.7%
Traditional
Defense
117/127
92.1%
45/51
88.2%
82/115
71.3%
244/293
83.2%
Overall Conviction rates by
Prosecution Method
124/134
92.5%
105/118
88.9%
236/305
77.4%
485/577
84.1%
* #convictions / #cases
The second hypothesis predicted that the prosecution strategy explicit politicality and the
defense strategy disassociation would produce the lowest conviction rates. The results indicated
some support for the hypothesis, but one prosecution strategy and defense method produced a
lower conviction rate than hypothesized. Explicit politicality and disassociation were both used
23
An attempt was made to test the interaction effects for each combination of prosecution method and defense
method using traditional defense and conventional criminality as reference categories. Multicolinarity was an
issue between political innuendo and explicit politicality (.642, p < .05), so the categories were reduced further,
into politicized prosecution strategy (political innuendo X explicit politicality) and politicized defense strategy
(political persecution X disassociation). Unfortunately, a crosstab revealed only 7 cases existed (out of 99) when
a politicized defense method was combined conventional criminality. The variance, then, was too small to
perform logistic regression.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
54
in 122 cases, and the combination produced a conviction rate of 79.5 percent. That is nearly 5
percent lower than the model average. In fact, explicit politicality produced the lowest
conviction rates (77.4%) of all prosecution strategies, and disassociation was the most successful
defense strategy (80.7%) used. Explicit politicality and a traditional defense strategy produced a
lower conviction rate of 71.3 percent.
Explicit politicality was the most frequently used prosecution strategy in the sample
(N=305). It only produced a conviction rate 7 percent below the model average (77.4%
compared to 84.1%). Even when used with the least successful defense strategy, political
persecution, explicit politicality produced convictions below the model average (83.8% to
84.1%). That result is remarkable considering that political persecution is the only defense
strategy, regardless of the prosecution strategy used, to result in a higher conviction rate (88.3%)
than the overall model average. Political persecution, overall, was the least successful defense
strategy. When used, political persecution produced a conviction rate of 88.3 percent. That is
well above the overall model conviction rate.
The disassociation defense strategy appears to be a more promising defense strategy for
defendants. The conviction rate for the disassociation defense, regardless of prosecutorial
method used, was only 80.7 percent, 3.4 percent below the overall model average. Similarly,
defendants using a traditional defense were slightly better off than the overall model average, as
they were convicted 0.9 percent less than the model average and significantly less than
defendants using the political persecution defense.
The examination turned to logistic regression in order to determine the effect that
prosecution strategies and defense strategies have on the odds being convicted. Because the
dependant variable, conviction, is dichotomous (0 = no, 1 = yes), binary logistic regression was
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
55
used. Recall from Chapter 3 that prosecution strategy (prosmeth) is coded as a categorical
variable with three categories. To test the effect of prosecution methods against each other, then,
each category was coded into a dummy variables, as follows:
Table 4.02
Coding scheme for Prosecution Strategy
conventional criminality prosmeth (0=no, 1=yes)
political innuendo prosmeth (0=no, 1=yes)
explicit politicality prosmeth (0=no, 1=yes)
Similarly, defense strategy (defmeth) is also a categorical variable with three categories. Defense
strategy was coded into three dummy variables as follows:
Table 4.03
Coding scheme for Defense Strategy
traditional defmeth (0=no, 1=yes)
disassociation defmeth (0=no, 1=yes)
political persecution defmeth (0=no, 1=yes)
For the first regression, the prosecution strategy variables for political innuendo and
explicit politicality were plugged into the model. The variable for conventional criminality was
withheld to serve as the comparison. The defense strategy variables for diffusion and political
persecution were included in the model, while the traditional defense variable was withheld to
serve as the comparison. Some additional predictor variables were added based on the literature
(see Chapters 2 and 3). Among those entered were race (0 = non-white, 1 = white) and gender
(0 = female, 1 = male). The variable count severity (see Appendix 1 for coding) was included.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
56
The variable informant (0 = no, 1 = yes), which measures whether a confidential informant was
available in a case, was used as a surrogate (though imperfect) measure of evidentiary strength.
The first regression produced a Nagelkerke R
2
of .105, but the variables race, gender, and
informant were not significant. The same model was performed with those variables removed.
The R
2
improved very slightly to.109, indicating a stronger model. For the sake of model
parsimony, the second set of results, presented in Table 4.03, were retained.
Table 4.04
Logistic Regression for Conviction by prosecutions strategy and defense strategy
Variables
Coefficients
Odds Ratio / exp(B)
(sig)
Political innuendo prosmeth
-1.179
.308
(.053)
Explicit politicality prosmeth
-1.834
.160
(.002)
Count severity
-.032
.969
(.065)
Political persecution defmeth
.653
1.920
(.081)
Disassociation defmeth
.590
1.804
(.059)
X
2
= 30.233, df 5, p< .001
Nagelkerke R
2
=.109
The logistic regression presented in Table 4.04 produced a R
2
of .109, so almost 11
percent of the variance surrounding whether a defendant received a conviction is explained by
the model. Of the variables entered, explicit politicality prosmeth was significant at the 99
percent confidence level. Each of the remaining variables was significant at the 90 percent
confidence interval.
The variable political innuendo prosmeth had a negative coefficient and the odds ratio
was < 1, indicating a negative relationship between using the political innuendo prosecution
method and the likelihood of being convicted. That means in cases for defendants who were
prosecuted using a political innuendo strategy (as compared to conventional criminality), the
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
57
odds of not being convicted (because 0=not convicted, 1 = convicted and the latter is being
modeled) increased by a factor of .308, controlling the effects of the other variables in the model.
Similarly, explicit politicality prosmeth, had a negative coefficient and the odds ratio was
less than one. In cases where defendants were prosecuted using an explicit politicality strategy
(as compared to conventional criminality), their odds of not being convicted increased by a
factor of .160, controlling the effects of the other variables in the model. The variable count
severity also indicates a negative relationship with the likelihood of conviction. Count severity is
an ordinal variable, measured from lowest to highest severity (1 to 29). For each increment that
count severity increases, the odds that a defendant will not be convicted increases by a factor of
.969, controlling the effects of the other variables in the model.
The predictor political persecution defmeth produced a positive coefficient and an odds
ratio of less than one, indicating a positive relationship between the use of the political
persecution defense strategy and receiving a conviction. Defendants who employed a political
persecution defense (compared to those who used a traditional defense strategy) increased their
odds of conviction by 1.920, controlling the effects of the other variables in the model.
Compared to using a traditional defense strategy, defendants who used a disassociation defense
strategy also had increased odds of conviction. The coefficient for disassociation defmeth was
positive and the odds ratio was greater than one. That means that defendants who used
disassociation (compared to a traditional defense) increased their odds of conviction by 1.804,
controlling the effects of other variables in the model.
A second binary logistic regression was run to determine whether the political innuendo
prosecution strategy effected the likelihood of conviction (compared to explicit politicality), and
to determine whether the disassociation defense strategy effected the likelihood of conviction
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
58
(compared to the political persecution strategy). In order to do this, conventional criminality
prosmeth was included in the model and explicit politicality prosmeth was removed for
comparison. For defense strategies, traditional defmeth was included and political persecutoin
defmeh was removed for comparison. This logistic regression produced a Nagelkerke R
2
of .109:
the model explains approximately 11 percent of the variance associated with whether a defendant
is convicted.
Table 4.05
Logistic Regression for Conviction by prosecutions strategy and defense strategy
Variables
Coefficients
Odds Ratio / exp(B)
(sig)
Conventional criminality prosmeth
1.834
6.257
(.002)
Political innuendo prosmeth
.655
1.925
(.061)
Count severity
-.032
.969
(.065)
Traditional defmeth
-.653
.521
(.081)
Disassociation defmeth
-.062
.939
(.874)
X
2
= 30.233, df 5, p< .001
Nagelkerke R
2
=.109
The result of the logistic regression, presented in Table 4.05, indicate that political
innuendo prosmeth is significant at a 90 percent confidence interval (p= .061). This is above the
generally accepted .05 level, but just slightly. The coefficient was positive and the odds ratio
was >1. This means, compared to the explicit politicality prosecution strategy, the odds of
conviction increased by 1.925 for defendants prosecuted with a political innuendo strategy,
controlling the effects of the other variables in the model. While disassociation defmeth
(compared to political persecution deftmeth) produced a negative coefficient and an odds ratio of
<1, it was statistically insignificant (p=.874). That means there is not a statistically significant
difference in the odds of being convicted for defendants who used either strategy.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
59
2. RESEARCH QUESTION TWO
The next task was to determine how 9/11 impacted the federal government’s response to
terrorism. Recall that data from the year 2001 were not included in these analyses. We divided
the remaining population of terrorist defendants into a pre-9/11 sample (consisting of all
database cases filed prior to 2001) and a post-9/11 sample (consisting of cases filed in and after
2002). For some analyses, we divided the cases in both eras by case type. Before any
hypotheses were tested, we ran descriptive statistics on the “case type” variable.
2.a. CASE TYPE DESCRIPTIVE STATISTICS
We first determined the overall frequencies for case type with the year 2001 deleted.
Case outcomes for 679 defendants were evaluated. Information was missing for 85 defendants—
most of the missing defendants were fugitive, awaiting trial, or had been transferred to another
jurisdiction—leaving 594 valid cases. There were a total of 481 defendants indicted in event-
linked cases in the sample. There were 119 defendants indicted in pretextual cases, and 79
defendants indicted in diffusion cases. Table 4.06 provides the distribution of frequencies
Table 4.06
Case Type Frequencies pre- and post-9/11
Case Type
Pre-9/11 Post-9/11
Total
Diffusion
0 79 79
Pretextual
81 38 119
Event-linked
431 50 481
Total
512 167 679
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
60
between the pre- and post-9/11 samples. In the pre-9/11 sample there were no diffusion cases,
but there were 81 (15.8%) pretextual cases and 431 (84.2%) event linked cases. In the post-9/11
sample there were 79 (47.3%) diffusion cases, 38 (22.8%) pretextual cases and 50 (29.9%)
event-linked cases.
Because they are categorical variables, we ran a crosstab between case_type and
outcome_recode, to determine the frequencies between case types and case outcomes for both
samples. Those results are presented in Table 4.07 (pre-9/11) and Table 4.08 (post-9/11). The
pre-9/11 model (n = 450) produced a significant chi-square(X
2
= 39.0, df 3, p <.001), revealing
that the variation between expected counts and observed counts across the model is substantial
and there is less than one chance in a thousand that these results occurred randomly.
Table 4.07
Crosstab Case Type by Outcome Pre-9/11
Case Outcome
Case Type
trial
conviction plea guilty
acquitted
at trial dismissed Total
Event-Linked
156
40.9%
149
39.1%
40
10.5%
36
9.4%
381
100.0%
Pretextual
8
11.6%
52
75.4%
0
.0%
9
13.0%
69
100.0%
Total
164
36.4%
201
44.7%
40
8.9%
45
10.0%
450
100.0%
X
2
= 39.0, df 3, p <.001
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
61
Table 4.08
Crosstab Case Type by Outcome Post-9/11
Case Outcome
Case Type
trial
conviction plea guilty
acquitted
at trial dismissed Total
Event-Linked
5
12.5%
28
70.0%
2
5.0%
5
12.5%
40
100.0%
Pretextual
3
11.1%
22
81.5%
0
.0%
2
7.4%
27
100.0%
Diffusion
5
6.5%
68
88.3%
1
1.3%
3
3.9%
77
100.0%
Total
13
9.0%
118
81.9%
3
2.1%
10
6.9%
144
100.0%
X
2
= 7.53, df 6, p =.275
Running the post-9/11 sample (n = 167) through the same model (see Table 4.06),
produced an insignificant chi-square (X
2
= 7.53, df 6, p =.275). The significance level is
probably the result of the missing cases (n = 23) and the smaller sample size. When the system
missing variables were recoded into a fifth category (5 = pending/fugitive), the model produced a
large and significant chi-square (X
2
= 24.763, df 8, p =.002). The large chi-square reveals that
there is significant variation between the expected case outcomes by case type, and that there
only two chances in a thousand (when the 5
th
category is added) that these results would occur
randomly. For the sake of clarity, the first model, with only four outcomes, is presented below.
For reference, the five-outcome model is provided in Appendix 9.
As Table 4.07 indicates, there were no diffusion cases filed in the pre-9/11 era. That left
381 event-linked cases (85%) and 69 pretextual cases (15%). In the post-9/11 sample there were
40 event-linked cases (28%) and 27 pretextual cases (19%). The majority of cases filed after
9/11 were diffusion cases; there were 77 diffusion cases making up 53 percent of the sample.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
62
In the pre-9/11 sample, 36.4 percent of the defendants were convicted at trial, and another
44.7 percent entered a guilty plea. Of the remaining defendants, 8.9 percent were acquitted of all
charges and 10 percent saw all charges against them dropped. The numbers were dramatically
different in the post-9/11 sample, where 82 percent of the defendants entered a plea of guilty.
After 9/11, only 9 percent of the defendants went to trial, another 2.1 percent were acquitted, and
charges were dropped against the remaining 6.9 percent (see Table 4.08).
The dramatic increase in the percentage of defendants who entered a guilty plea was
among the more interesting trends. The plea rate increased 37.2 percent for defendants indicted
after 9/11. As a result, the percentage of defendants going to trial was lower by 27.4 percent.
One would expect that as fewer cases go to trial, fewer defendants are acquitted; that was the
case here. The acquittal rate dropped by 6.8 percent after 9/11. Finally, the number of cases
where all charges were dropped against a defendant dropped slightly, from 10 percent to 6.9
percent.
Another interesting trend was the drop in event-linked cases: 85 percent of the defendants
in the pre-9/11 era were tied to a terrorist group/ideology and they were charged with attacking
or attempting to attack a target. That number dropped to just 28 percent of cases after 9/11.
Prosecutors filed the first diffusion cases in the post-9/11 era. Importantly, diffusion cases make
up 53 percent of the cases filed after 9/11.
Next, we ran a crosstab between case_type and pros_ meth, to determine the frequencies
between case types and prosecution strategies. The preliminary model for the full database (both
pre- and post-9/11 samples, without cases from 2001) produced an enormous and highly
significant chi-square (X
2
= 315.0, df 4, p < 001). The high chi-square statistic is not surprising.
Case type and prosecution method are highly correlated (Pearson correlation = .624, p < .001).
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
63
There is a strong relationship between the type of case filed, and the prosecution strategy used to
pursue it.
Because the preliminary model indicated a very strong relationship between case type
and prosecution method, the same model was run for the pre-9/11 sample and then again for
post-9/11 sample. As Table 4.09 shows, there were a total of 511 defendants in the pre-9/11
model and it produced a very high and very significant chi-square (X
2
= 88.7, df 2, p < 001).
Table 4.09
Crosstab Case Type by Prosecution Strategy Pre-9/11
Prosecution Strategy
Case Type
conventional
criminality
political
innuendo
explicit
politicality Total
Event-Linked
24
5.6%
101
23.5%
305
70.9%
430
100.0%
Pretextual
33
40.7%
15
18.5%
33
40.7%
81
100.0%
Total
57
11.2%
116
22.7%
338
66.1%
511
100.0%
X
2
= 88.7, df 2, p < 001
Table 4.10
Crosstab Case Type by Prosecution Strategy Post-9/11
Prosecution Strategy
Case Type
conventional
criminality
political
innuendo
explicit
politicality Total
Event-Linked
5
10.0%
15
30.0%
30
60.0%
50
100.0%
Pretextual
11
29.7%
16
43.2%
10
27.1%
37
100.0%
Diffusion
71
89.8%
6
7.6%
2
2.6%
79
100.0%
Total
87
52.4%
37
22.3%
42
25.3%
166
100.0%
X
2
= 98.2, df 4, p <.001
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
64
That suggests that there was tremendous variation between expected and observed counts in each
of the cells, and there is less than one chance in a thousand that the variation occurred randomly.
Table 4.10 contains the results for the post-9/11 sample (n = 166). The post-9/11 chi-
square indicates a very large and significant amount of variation between expected and observed
counts (X
2
= 98.2, df 4, p < 001). Prosecutors tried 66.1 percent of the pre-9/11 defendants using
an explicit politicality prosecution strategy, 22.7 percent were tried by political innuendo, and
just 11.2 percent by conventional criminality. After 9/11, prosecutors tried the majority of the
defendants, 52.4 percent, with a conventional criminality strategy, 22.3 percent were tried by
political innuendo, and 25.3 percent were tried using the explicit politicality strategy.
The largest single factor accounting for the drop in the percentage of explicit politicality
prosecutions would appear to be the inclusion of diffusion cases in the post-9/11 era. However,
even after removing the diffusion category the percentage of explicit politicality cases was 60
percent, a decrease of 10.9 percent from the pre-9/11 era. Prosecutors used the conventional
criminality strategy in 52.4 percent of the post-9/11cases. That is an increase of 41.2 percent.
Removing diffusion cases from the model reduced the percentage to 18.4 percent, but that still
means that prosecutors relied on this strategy 7.2 percent more often than in the previous era.
Finally, prosecutors used the political innuendo strategy in 22.3 percent of the cases they filed in
the post-9/11 era. That is a 0.4 percent decline from the pre-9/11 era. But when diffusion cases
were removed, the percentage use of conventional criminality actually increased by 12.9 percent
(to 35.6 %).
Among defendants indicted in event-linked cases before 9/11, 70.9 percent were
prosecuted using an explicit politicality strategy, 23.5 percent were prosecuted using political
innuendo, and 5.6 percent where prosecuted using conventional criminality. After 9/11, the
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
65
percentage of event-linked prosecutions relying on an explicit politicality strategy dropped to 60
percent. The percentage of conventional criminality prosecutions increased to 10 percent, and
political innuendo cases increased to 30 percent.
In the pretextual category, results from the pre-9/11 model show that defendants were
prosecuted using both conventional criminality and explicit politicality equally (40.7 percent
each). The remaining 18.5 percent of all pretextual cases were prosecuted using the political
innuendo strategy. In the post-9/11 sample, the use of political innuendo increased to 43.2
percent, up 24.7 percent from the pre-9/11 level. During the same time frame, the use of explicit
politicality decreased to 27 percent, and the use of conventional criminality decreased to 29.7
percent.
For the pre-9/11 model, the largest portion of the chi-square was explained in four cells.
The cell for pretextual case type and conventional criminality prosecution methods produced an
expected frequency of 9 and an observed frequency of 33—an increase of 24 cases (266%). The
observed frequency for pretextual case type and explicit politicality prosecution strategy was
lower than the expect frequency by 23 cases (-41.1%). The cell for event-linked case type and
conventional criminality prosecution strategy had an lower observed frequency by 24 cases—a
decrease of 24 cases (-50%). The observed frequency for event-linked case type and explicit
politicality prosecution strategy was higher than expected by 11 cases (7.3%). The two
remaining cells differed in observed versus expected frequencies by less than 4 cases.
In the post-9/11 model, the cells for diffusion case type and each of the prosecution
strategies produced significant variance. The variance between observed and expected
frequencies for diffusion case type and conventional criminality differed the most (+30 cases,
73%), followed by explicit politicality (-18 cases, -90%), and then political innuendo (-11 cases,
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
66
-64%). The cell containing event-linked case type and conventional criminality produced 21
fewer cases than expected(-81%). The observed frequency for event linked case type and
explicit politicality was higher than expected by 17 cases (17%). The remaining cells produced
variation between expected and observed frequencies of less than 9 cases.
For the next analyses, we substituted prosecution method with defense method
(def_meth) and ran the same models. The number of defendants remained the same in both sets
of analyses. The pre-9/11 model produced a chi-square of 12.47 (df 2, p = .002) and the post-
9/11 model produced a chi-square of 31.5 (df 4, p <.001). The chi-square statistic for both
models were large and significant at the 99 percent confidence interval. Meaning, there is a
significant relationship between defense strategy and case type. As compared to the prosecution
strategy models, though, the lower chi-square statistics reveal that defense strategies are not
correlated as strongly to case type.
The results for the pre-9/11 crosstab are presented in Table 4.11. The overall
frequencies indicate that 44.8 percent of the defendants in this sample used a traditional defense
strategy to avoid conviction, 34.5 percent of the defendants relied on a disassociation strategy,
and 20.7 percent employed political persecution.
Table 4.11
Crosstab Case Type by Defense Strategy Pre-9/11
Defense Strategy
Case Type
Political
Persecution Disassociation Traditional Total
Event-Linked
88
23.8%
125
33.8%
157
42.4%
370
100.0%
Pretextual
5
6.3%
30
38.0%
44
55.7%
79
100.0%
Total
93
20.7%
155
34.5%
201
44.8%
449
100.0%
X
2
= 12.5, df 2, p = 002
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
67
In the post-9/11 model (see Table 4.12), 73.1 percent of the defendants used a
conventional defense strategy and 13.1 percent used the disassociation strategy. The remaining
13.8 percent of the defendants relied on political persecution. That means after 9/11 the
percentage of defendants using a traditional defense increased by 28.3 percent. If diffusion cases
are removed from the model, the percentage of defendants using a traditional defense drops to
53.5 percent. That is still an increase of 8.7 percent.
Table 4.12
Crosstab Case Type by Defense Strategy Post-9/11
Defense Strategy
Case Type
Political
Persecution Disassociation Traditional Total
Event-Linked
7
20.6%
8
23.5%
19
55.9%
34
100.0%
Pretextual
12
32.4%
6
16.2%
19
51.4%
37
100.0%
Diffusion
1
1.4%
5
6.8%
68
91.9%
74
100.0%
Total
20
13.8%
19
13.1%
106
73.1%
145
100.0%
X
2
= 31.5, df 4, p < 001
The proportion of defendants using a disassociation defense strategy decreased by 21.4
percent in the post-9/11 era. When diffusion cases were removed, the percentage of defendants
using disassociation increased to 19.7 percent, but that is still an overall decrease of 14.8 percent
from the pre-9/11 model. Table 4.12 also shows a decrease in the number of defendants using
the political persecution defense strategy from the pre-9/11 model to the post-9/11 model. After
9/11, only 13.8 percent of the defendants relied on political persecution. However, when
diffusion cases were removed the results show an increase of 6.1 percent for a total of 26.8
percent.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
68
When considering case type, some noteworthy trends emerge. Of the defendants who
were prosecuted in event-linked cases, fewer used political persecution and disassociation as
defense strategies after 9/11 than before. The percentage of defendants using political
persecution dropped slightly from 23.8 to 20.6 (although this change is not significant), and the
proportion of defendants using disassociation changed from 33.8 to 23.5 percent. The
percentage of defendants in event-linked cases who relied on a traditional defense increased from
42.4 to 55.9 - an increase of 13.5 percent.
The trends were different for defendants who were indicted in pretextual cases. Fewer
defendants in the post-9/11 era used a traditional defense. Before 9/11, defendants in pretextual
cases used a traditional defense 55.7 percent of the time, but afterwards, that percentage dropped
to 51.4 percent. More dramatically, 38 percent of the defendants used a disassociation defense
before 9/11, but only 16.2 percent used that defense afterwards. The percentage of defendants
using a political persecution strategy increased from 6.3 to 32.4 in the post-9/11 era. Finally, we
will note that in diffusion cases, 91.9 percent of the defendants used a traditional defense.
Similarly, 89.9 percent of defendants were tried using a conventional criminality prosecution
strategy. The discussion now turns to the results of hypothesis testing.
2.b.
TESTS OF HYPOTHESES
Hypothesis 3 predicted that, if structural contextual theory is correct and tightened
coupling has occurred among components of the criminal justice system, prosecutors would
voluntarily dismiss fewer cases/counts than in the post-9/11 era. There are two ways to measure
dismissal: cases dismissed and counts dismissed (i.e. charge bargaining) from cases.
Independent samples t-tests were used for both analyses. The results for case dismissals are
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
69
found in Table 4.13, and indicate that there was a statistically significant (p <.001) decrease in
the proportion of cases dismissed from the pre-9/11 era (.09, SD = .285) to the post-9/11 era (.02,
SD = .143). However, if diffusion cases are removed from the model, the results show no
significant difference between eras.
Table 4.13
Number of Cases Dismissed Pre- and Post-9/11
Era proportion N Std. Deviation
Pre-9/11
.09 450 .285
Post-9/11
.02 144 .143
t(466)= 3.8, p < .001
The results for counts dismissed per case were not as expected. As Table 4.14 shows, the
number of counts dismissed increased. While the average number of counts appears to have
decreased, the t-test indicates an extremely high probability that the difference between eras is
not statistically significant. The results do not support this hypothesis.
Table 4.14
Number of Dismissed Counts per Case Pre- and Post-9/11
Era proportion N Std. Deviation
Pre-9/11
1.37 402 1.37
Post-9/11
1.17 138 1.17
t(538)= .339, p = .735
Hypothesis 4 predicted that if tightened coupling had occurred, the percentage of plea
bargained cases would decrease among pretextual prosecutions in the post-9/11 era. The t-test
reveals that there is not a statistically significant (p = .382) difference in the guilty plea rate
between eras (see Table 4.15). Though the results are not statistically significant, they suggest
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
70
that the guilty plea rate may have increased between eras. The results do not support hypothesis
4.
Table 4.15
Percentage of Plea Bargains in Pretextual Cases
proportion N Std. Deviation
Pre-9/11
.69 35 .471
Post-9/11
.79 28 .418
t(61)= -.88, p = .382
Hypothesis 5 tested the hydraulic effect, and this hypothesis predicted that if net
widening had occurred, cases in the post-9/11 era would involve a higher proportion of low-level
offenses than in cases filed before 9/11. This analysis tested by independent samples t-test. The
average for severity was taken from the lead offense for each defendant in both samples. We
used lead offense because prosecutors normally list the counts in an indictment in order of
severity. The results are presented in Table 4.16. The t-test revealed a significant decrease in
average count severity between the pre-9/11 era (M =19.38, SD = 9.15) and the post-9/11 era (M
= 15.71, SD 9.321).
Table 4.16
Average Count Severity before and after 9/11
Era Mean N Std. Deviation
Pre-9/11
19.38 403 9.149
Post-9/11
15.71 153 9.321
t(554) = 4.20, p < .001
When diffusion cases were removed from the analysis (model not shown) to compare
only pretextual and event-linked cases from both eras, the results were not significant t(134) =
.180, p = .858. That means, when looking at similar case types between eras, there was not a
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
71
statistically significant change in count severity. Results for the overall model (all case-types)
support the hypothesis, but results from the second model do not.
Hypothesis 6 predicted that, if net widening has occurred, there would be a higher
proportion of pretextual prosecutions compared to event-linked prosecutions in the post-9/11 era.
We removed the diffusion cases from the samples, and tested the hypothesis with two analyses.
In the first analysis, shown in Table 4.17, we analyzed the total number of defendants using
crosstabs (indictee level analysis). In the second analysis, shown in Table 4.18, we analyzed the
proportion of pretextual cases in each era using crosstabs (case level analysis).
Table 4.17
Crosstab Defendants in Pretextual Cases
Case Type
Total
ERA
Event -Linked Pretextual
Pre-9/11
431
84.2%
81
15.8%
512
100.0%
Post-9/11
50
56.8%
38
43.2%
88
100.0%
Total
481
80.2%
119
19.8%
600
100.0%
X
2
= 35.4, df 1, p <.001
As can be seen in Table 4.17, the chi-square was large and significant (X
2
= 35.4, df 1, p
<.001). There were a total of 512 defendants in the pre-9/11 sample and 88 in post-9/11 sample.
Of all the defendants who were indicted in the pre-911 sample, 15.8 percent were indicted in
pretextual cases. As predicted, the percentage of defendants indicted in pretextual cases
increased 27.4 percent to 43.2 in the post-9/11 era.
We found a similar trend in the second model. There were a total of 209 cases in this
model: 157 in the pre-9/11 sample and 52 in the post-9/11 sample. As the results in Table 4.18
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
72
show, before 9/11, only 12.1 percent of the cases filed were pretextual. That percentage
increased by 22.1 percent for a total of 44.2 in the post-9/11 era.
Table 4.18
Crosstab Pretextual Cases
Total
Case Type
Event -Linked Pretextual
Pre-9/11
138
87.9%
19
12.1%
157
100.0%
Post-9/11
29
55.8%
23
44.2%
52
100.0%
Total
167
79.9%
42
20.1%
209
100.0%
X
2
= 25.1, df 1, p <.001
Hypothesis 7 predicted that, in event-linked cases, the total number of defendants per
cases would be smaller in the post-9/11 era. After limiting the database to event-linked cases, we
ran an independent samples t-test to compare the means for the average number of defendants
per case in both the pre- and post-9/11 samples. The results, presented in Table 4.19, are
statistically significant (p = .001). That means there was a significant drop in the average
number of defendants in event-linked cases between the pre- and post-9/11 eras. The results
support Hypothesis 7.
Table 4.19
Average Number of Defendants in Event-linked Cases
Era Proportion N Std. Deviation
Pre-9 /11
3.43 138 4.278
Post-9/11
1.72 29 1.667
t(114) = 3.56, p = .001
Hypothesis 8 predicted that the proportion of event-linked cases involving informants
would be smaller in the post-9/11 era. We planned to use six different models to test this
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
73
hypothesis, but as we will explain below, we ultimately ran four. In the first, we determined the
proportion of cases using confidential informants in the first model using the dichotomous
variable, informant (0 = no informant, 1 = had informant) in an independent samples t-test. The
results in Table 4.20 show that the proportion of cases that involved confidential informants
decreased significantly between the pre/911 era (58%) and the post-9/11 era (20%). The results
support hypothesis 8.
Table 4.20
Proportion of Cases with Confidential Informant
Era Proportion N Std. Deviation
Pre-9 /11
.58 120 .886
Post-9/11
.20 25 .408
t(40.2)= 4.02, p < .001
The results for undercover agents are presented in Table 4.21. As with confidential
informants, the proportion of cases that made use of an undercover agent decreased dramatically.
Undercover agents were used in 31 percent of cases filed before 9/11, and in only 4 percent of
case filed afterwards (p < .001). That is a decrease of 27 percent after 9/11. These findings
support Hypothesis 8, but we must note that there was only one event-linked case in the post-
9/11 era that involved an undercover agent so the results of the t-test were not significant. We
will nonetheless present these findings for discussion.
Table 4.21
Proportion of Cases with Undercover Agents
Era Proportion N Std. Deviation
Pre-9 /11
.31 118 .462
Post-9/11
.04 25 .200
t(86.4) = 4.5, p < .001
Among cases where at least one confidential informant was used, the average number of
informants per case decreased after 9/11. The model was statistically significant. The results in
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
74
Table 4.22 indicate that before 9/11 the prosecution used an average of 4 informants in these
cases. After 9/11 the average decreased to 1.2. The model testing the number of undercover
agents is not presented here. Since there was only one case in the post-9/11 era that made use of
an undercover agent we chose not to include the results. We will note, however, that in event-
linked cases filed prior to 9/11, there was an average of 2.67 agents in cases in which at least one
undercover agent was used.
Table 4.22
Number of Confidential Informants per Case
Era Mean N Std. Deviation
Pre-9 /11
4.00 55 13.264
Post-9/11
1.20 5 .447
T (35) = .895, p = .377.
In the final model, we determined the average amount of assistance provided by
confidential informants before and after 9/11. Those results are presented in Table 4.23. The
results are significant at a .10 level. As a reminder, the variable we used was infor_level. It is an
ordinal variable coded from 1 to 4 (low to high). The results show that the level of assistance
provided by confidential informants in event-linked cases decreased after 9/11 by .37. These
results support Hypothesis 8.
Table 4.23
Average Level of Assistance Provided by Confidential Informants per Case
Era Mean N Std. Deviation
Pre 9/11
2.71 51 1.113
Post-9/11
2.00 5 .707
T(6) = 2.7, p < .05.
Hypothesis 9 predicted that conviction rates on more serious charges would decrease
after 9/11. We ran two bivariate analyses. Count severity was recoded into a dichotomous
variable divided on the mean (19), and independent sample t-tests were run. The first column in
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
75
Table 4.24 shows the results for the pre/11 sample. The difference in means is statistically
significant and indicates that low severity crimes resulted in conviction approximately 88 percent
of the time, while high severity crimes resulted in conviction at a lower rate of 76 percent.
Conversely, results in the second column (post-9/11) were not statically significant. That means
in the post-9/11 era, there was not a statistically significant difference in conviction rates
between high and low severity offenses. Comparing two models, one will also note that
conviction rates were higher, regardless of severity, in the post-9/11 era. The results do not
support Hypothesis 9.
Table 4.24
Conviction Rate by Count Severity
Conviction rate
Severity Pre-9/11* Mean Post-9/11**
Low Severity .88
(sd = .331)
.90
(sd = .331)
High Severity .76
(sd = ..429)
.92
(sd = ..429)
* p = .005
**p = .75
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
76
V. PROSECUTORIAL AND DEFENSE STRATEGIES IN TERRORISM CASES
The discussion first focuses on the general outcomes and characteristics of prosecution
strategies. In this section, explicit politicality emerges as a very risky prosecution strategy when
compared to its alternatives. In the next section, the discussion turns to defense strategies. One
important finding presented is that the disassociation defense strategy produces the lowest
conviction rates overall, but its success proves to be situational. Another finding presented is the
positive impact the political persecution defense strategy has on the likelihood of conviction.
Finally, results of the hypothesis testing are presented along with explanations on why some
prosecutorial and defense strategy combinations produce high conviction rates, while others do
not.
1. GENERAL PROSECUTION STRATEGIES
In Chapter 2, we argued that terrorism cases are very different from traditional criminal
cases in that prosecutors have developed different strategies to prosecute them. But then in
Chapter 4, we reported that 23 percent of all terrorism cases are treated no differently than
traditional criminal cases. This begs the question: do either the prosecutors or the defendants
know that the investigation was conducted under a terrorism enterprise investigation by the FBI?
We believe they do.
Case documents reveal that many defendants are aware that they were either targeted
prior to arrest, or investigated after arrest, by law enforcement officials working for, or with, a
Joint Terrorism Task Force (“JTTF”). Former Assistant United States Attorney Robert Mclean,
who prosecuted cases in the Northern District of Alabama, agrees. During a 2007 interview, he
stated that it is common practice for defendants to become aware of JTTF involvement during
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
77
the discovery process (the phase of a court case where the government must provide the
defendant with all the evidence it intends to use to prove its case). Moreover, McLean suggested
that prosecutors always know when a case is referred by the FBI, and they know whether it was
subject to a terrorism investigation. The use of a conventional criminality prosecution strategy
would appear to be a conscious and carefully considered decision.
There are several reasons why prosecutors may use a conventional criminality
prosecution strategy over another approach. First, the cases typically involve simple charges
(e.g. strict liability charges) and may be easier to prove and require fewer resources than more
complex cases. In effect, a prosecutor may decide that it is better to get the defendant off the
street on a low-severity charge that can be proved, than to risk acquittal on a more severe charge
where the evidence is questionable. On a scale of increasing severity from 1 to 28, the average
count severity for conventional criminality cases in this study was 17.81, compared to 18.20 for
political innuendo and 23.27 for explicit politicality (see appendix 2). In essence, the more
serious the charges, the more likely defendants will be explicitly labeled as terrorists.
Second, prosecutors may have little choice. Rule 403 of the Federal Rules of Evidence
(FRE 403) was written to prevent the use of prejudicial evidence in court cases where that
evidence does not aid in the determination of guilt. For example, if the only charge prosecutors
believe can be proven is related to narcotics possession, they may feel they do not have legal
standing to introduce evidence that the defendant was sympathetic to the white supremacy
movement. Third, prosecutors may want to avoid giving the defendant a stage from which to
advocate his or her cause. As mentioned in Chapter 2, Terrorism cases can garner tremendous
attention from the press.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
78
Regardless of the reason for using it, conventional criminality was the most successful
prosecution strategy tested, generating a conviction rate of 92.5 percent. That is a much higher
conviction rate than when all other prosecution strategies were combined (84.1%). In fact, the
92.5 percent conviction rate more closely mirrors non-terrorist cases in the federal system (see
Chapter 2). The similarity is probably not a coincidence, as conventional criminality cases are
treated much like non-terrorist cases. For example, defendants may be charged with any number
of criminal violations, but at no time do those charges involve proving that the defendant was
engaged in attacking (or planning to attack) a target for political reasons.
It is also true of conventional criminality cases that prosecutors avoid any attempt to link
a defendant to a terrorist group or ideology. On occasion, prosecutors have fought to keep
politics out of their cases. For example, in US v Merrell, prosecutors wanted to avoid
politicizing the terrorist cause to the degree that they filed motions to block the defendant from
making any reference to his political affiliation (see US v Merrell, 96-CR-257 WFN, E.D.
Washington, 1996). But typically, if a prosecutor avoids politicizing a case, the defendants will
not force the issue. Recall, in Table 4.02, that prosecutors used a conventional criminality
strategy against 134 defendants from 1980 to 2004. During that time, only 7 defendants
attempted to use a politicized defense strategy and all ended up convicted.
As a prosecution strategy, political innuendo was almost as successful as conventional
criminality. The political innuendo strategy resulted in convictions 88.9 percent of the time,
versus 92.1 for the latter. Political innuendo cases typically involve less serious offenses than
explicit politicality cases, but slightly more serious charges than conventional criminality (see
Appendix 2). In total, prosecutors chose the political innuendo strategy over other prosecutorial
strategies 21 percent of the time.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
79
Like the conventional criminality strategy, political innuendo is characterized by charges
that resemble traditional criminal cases. While they involve conspiracy charges twice as often (6
percent compared to 3 percent) as conventional criminality cases, those charges do not involve
proving that the defendant was engaged in attacking, or planning to attack, a target for political
reasons. Recall that with the political innuendo strategy the prosecution attempts to link the
defendant to terrorism in cases where the defendant is charged with non-politicized criminal
violations. In some instances the prosecution provides only a subtle hint, but in others the
prosecution blatantly makes the claim.
For an example of using a subtle hint, consider US v Norris (85 CR 0010, U.S. District
Court for the Northern District of Alabama). In the Norris case, the prosecution mentioned the
group “The Order” and implicated the defendants in hiding notorious Order member, Robert
Matthews, but the defendants were not called terrorists in the record, and the prosecution did not
claim they were members of the Order. For an example of more blatant reference, consider the
case of US v Fernandez (84 CR 0134, U.S. District Court for the Southern District of New
York). In a prior case, Eduardo Arocena (Omega 7) decided to cooperate with the government.
Arocena told authorities that Defendant Fernandez had taken part in one machine gun murder,
and that Omega 7 used Fernandez’s store to construct bombs used in an attempted car bombing
of a Cuban Ambassador. The government offered Fernandez immunity and ordered him to
testify before the Grand Jury. He refused, and was held in civil contempt for 18 months until the
United States brought charges against him in the above case. While the indictment read like a
simple “failure to appear,” at Fernandez’s bond hearing, the AUSA called the defendant a
terrorist and recounted the above information in open court.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
80
Not only do prosecutors expose a defendant’s link to terrorism in different ways, subtly
or blatantly, prosecutors appear to do so for different reasons and at different points during a
case. Sometimes the connection between a defendant and a terrorism group happens at the
sentencing phase of a case in what is likely a calculated move to increase the sentence imposed.
In US v Walid (87 CR 78, U.S. District Court for the District of Vermont), the defendants were
linked to explosive devices found near a railroad track where they had been seen. From the
opening statements to the closing arguments, no mention of the word terrorism, political
ideology, or an act of terrorism appeared in the record. That changed during the sentencing
phase. The sentencing memorandum not only asserted a connection between the defendants and
a group, the Syrian Social Nationalist Party, but it defined terrorism and made a political
argument about the dangers of terrorism. In fact, an affidavit attached to the sentencing
memorandum provided a history of the SSNP since 1930.
In other cases, the government may allege the defendant’s membership in a notorious
group to sway the jury. Such was the case in US v Schweitzer (95-CR-117, U.S. District Court
for the District of Montana). In this case, the government charged the defendants with
conspiracy to commit fraud on banks and the government, but never mentioned the political
purposes of the defendants. Instead, prosecutors simply told the jurors that the defendants were
members of a notorious group, the Freemen. It is clear from the court record that the Freeman
were well known in the media and among the jury members.
Prosecutors have also used the political innuendo strategy very early in a case to prevent
defendants from being released on bail. In the Fernandez case above, the record shows that the
prosecutor asked that bail be set at a level that the defendant could not pay. After hearing about
the defendant’s possible connection to terrorism, the Magistrate said on the record that $500,000
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
81
was probably beyond what the defendant could raise, but if he should find it, the AUSA could
advise the court and the court would reconsider the amount. In US v Siddiqui (01-CR-393, U.S.
District Court for the District of North Carolina), the defendant was indicted on simple ID Fraud.
The indictment did not contain any information or allegations linking the defendant to terrorism,
but at the bond hearing, the prosecution raised the specter of the defendant’s potential
involvement with 9/11. He was denied bail on the grounds of being a danger to the community.
At one point, the defendant took a polygraph on his connection with 9/11 - it was inconclusive.
Regardless, the judge found that Siddiqui was a danger to the community and refused to grant his
bail, even before the INS had perfected an immigration hold.
In one more example, US V Budiman (02-CR-74, U.S. District Court for the Eastern
District of Virginia) the defendant was indicted for ID fraud. The indictment did not mention
any terrorist activity. At his arraignment, however, the prosecution suggested that Budiman
knew the 9/11 hijackers and may have further involvement with the attack. A court appointed
attorney, in open court, requested to be removed from the case immediately. After Budiman was
appointed a new attorney, the government asked that he not be released on bail because he was a
flight risk. Again, the assertion that he may have been associated with the 9/11 hijackers was
raised. Without addressing the flight risk issue, the court found that he posed a danger to the
community and denied his bail.
Unlike when using the political innuendo strategy, explicit politicality makes the
defendant’s political motive a core component of the case. Cases in which the explicit
politicality strategy is used involve more serious charges than cases tried using the other
prosecution strategies (see Appendix 2). In addition, these cases are more complex, as 20
percent of all counts filed are conspiracy charges (see Appendix 3). Explicit politicality was also
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
82
the most frequently used prosecutorial strategy, accounting for almost 55 percent of all
prosecutions. Explicit politicality cases tend to draw the most media attention, and
unfortunately, that exposes prosecutors and law enforcement to the potential of bad press.
Explicit politicality is by far the least successful prosecution method, generating an overall
conviction rate of just 77 percent. As Tables 4.03 and 4.04 show, explicit politicality has a
statistically significant and strong negative impact on the likelihood of conviction. That
negativity continues to exist even when the effects of crime severity and case complexity are
controlled.
As previously mentioned, a defendant’s political motive is central to the explicit
politicality prosecution strategy, but even that is subject to nuances. These nuances may be the
result of prosecutors attempting to find a balance between politicizing a case and generating a
conviction. In US v Whitehorn (88 CR 0145, United States District Court for the District of
Washington, D.C.), the sentencing segment of the case was far more politicized than the previous
segments. Linda Evans and Laura Whitehorn were members of the May 19 Communist
Organization, a fact that the prosecutor mentioned repeatedly throughout the case. The
government’s sentencing memorandum goes much further. The prosecutor compares the
defendants’ actions to violent acts committed by the KKK and the Neo-Nazi’s. Interestingly, the
description of the defendants’ actions and goals are exceptionally close to the FBI’s definition of
terrorism.
Four years later, prosecutors in Arizona tried a different approach in a case filed against
members of the Provisional Irish Republican Army. In US v Maguire (92 CR 587, United States
District Court for the Northern District of Arizona), prosecutors attempted to politicize the case
while trying to prevent the defendants from doing the same. Prosecutors went to the Grand Jury
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
83
with a conspiracy case clearly identifying the Provisional Irish Republican Army and suggesting
that explosive materials recovered during the FBI investigation would be used to kill foreign
nationals. Fearing juror empathy, prosecutors then tried to block the defendants from bringing
up the political and religious history of the PIRA. At the same time, the defendants sought to
bring in PIRA history and, moreover, they wanted to introduce evidence of political persecution
perpetrated by the British Government. The court was in a conundrum over the competing
motions (nearly 270 motions filed in the case), but eventually it ruled that the government had
opened the door. The court allowed both sides to bring in their political evidence.
2. GENERAL DEFENSE STRATEGIES
Introducing politicality in terrorism cases adds to the contentiousness that already exists
in criminal trials. It also poses new challenges to defense attorneys trying to defend their clients.
To counter the strategies created by prosecutors, defense attorneys have tried a number of
strategies of their own. This study focused on the three most common. First, the disassociation
strategy was employed by defendants who tried to distance themselves from other group
members, extremist ideologies, or sometimes, simply the most politicized counts in a given
indictment. Second, the political persecution strategy was used by defendants who attempted to
claim that they were the subject of political crusades that had targeted them for having
alternative political views. And third, like prosecutors who avoid using a political prosecution
strategy, some defendants took their chances by avoiding the political issue altogether and relied
on a traditional defense strategy. It should be noted that “traditional” does not mean a particular
defense strategy has been employed, as there are hundreds of defense tactics. Rather it means
that the defendant has a defense strategy that ignores or avoids the political issues altogether.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
84
Over the past 20 years, the disassociation strategy has proven to be the most effective of
the three strategies for defendants to avoid conviction. In 20 percent of the cases where
disassociation has been used, defendants have been acquitted of all charges or have had all
charges against them dismissed. In addition to having all counts dropped or being acquitted on
all counts, sometimes a few counts from an indictment are dropped or result in conviction even
when a defendant is convicted on other counts. The table in appendix 4 shows that 74.3 percent
of all counts that resulted in acquittal (n=101) occurred when the defendant employed a
disassociation strategy.
In addition, counts are sometimes dropped when a prosecutor charges a defendant in a
superseding indictment. Sometimes called charge bargaining, counts that appeared in an original
or preceding indictment against a defendant are left off of a subsequent indictment. The table in
Appendix 5 shows that prosecutors dismiss counts much more often, nearly 2 counts per
defendant (1.91), when those defendants used a disassociation strategy than compared to
defendants who used other defense strategies (.19 counts per defendant in political persecution
cases, .77 counts per defendant in traditional defense case).
Disassociation occurs in several different ways. A common method is for a defendant to
file a motion in limine to sever his or her case from other defendants. This is particularly popular
among rank and file group members who have been indicted with group leaders or more
notorious members. Sometimes a defendant attempts to limit the government’s use of certain
terms (such as “terrorism” or “KKK”) that might connect a defendant to a particular ideology or
event. In other cases, defendants attempt to sever highly politicized charges from their
indictment, arguing that by including the politicized charges with more mundane counts, the
defendants are prejudiced. Finally, some events and some groups are so notorious in a particular
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
85
area that defendants believe that the jury pool is likely tainted, so they request not only a
severance, but also a change of venue.
Examples of each of these methods occurred in US v Schweitzer (95-CR-117, U.S.
District Court for the District of Montana). Defendant Dana Dudley filed a number of motions
to distance herself from the other defendants and from the group ideology. Her first was a
motion to limit verbiage—to prevent the government from using the term “compound” when
referring to the property on which the defendants lived. She filed a second motion to prohibit the
government from using the term “Freemen” when describing the defendants. Other defendants
in the Schweitzer case filed motions to sever their cases from one another, and several filed
motions to sever the robbery counts from the fraud counts. The Schweitzer jury found itself
hung on three counts during the first trial, so the government retried the defendants some 3
months later. During that time, the defendants filed dozens of motions to sever, basing some of
those motions on the proposed testimony of a proposed witness for another defendant. At least 5
defendants attempted to have the venue changed as they believed that the bad press about the
Freeman would prejudice the jury. As the second trial neared, two defendants tried again to limit
verbiage.
In an argument typical of defendants who attempt to limit verbiage, Dana Dudley relied
on Federal Rules of Evidence 403 (“FRE 403”). Dudley argued that the use of the words
“compound” and “Freemen” would introduce prejudice beyond any probative value. In effect,
she argued that the words themselves offered nothing that would help the jury decide her guilt,
but those words were likely to inflame jury members. The government responded to her motions
by arguing the nature of the defendants’ property was in fact a compound, and the government
pointed out that the defendants used the term “Freemen” to describe themselves. In the opinion
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
86
denying her motion, the court noted that Dudley had filed an earlier pro se petition requesting a
“White Christian Judge” and she signed the petition “a Free White Christian Woman.”
The political persecution defense strategy is another popular method used in terrorism
cases, invoked by 18 percent of the defendants in this study. Political persecution is similar to an
affirmative defense in that the defendant is arguing that he or she has been indicted because of
his or her political beliefs; that, in actuality, they have done nothing but live according to their
belief system and should not be punished for it. Political persecution is not a recognized
affirmative defense, such that the burden of proof shifts to the defendant to prove they are being
persecuted. But it nonetheless asks the jury to sympathize with their situation. The strategy is a
gamble that rarely works.
Defendants who rely on the political persecution strategy are convicted at the highest rate
of all defendants in this study, 88.3 percent of the time. As Appendix 4 indicates, those numbers
are even stronger when one looks at count outcomes rather than case outcomes. When using
political persecution as a defense strategy, 41 percent of all counts result in a jury conviction,
compared to 20.5 percent of all counts defended by disassociation. In other words, not only do
defendants get convicted more frequently, they get convicted on more counts.
It is also the case that defendants who employ this strategy are more likely to go to trial
and therefore, less likely to plead guilty. Similarly, prosecutors faced with a political persecution
defense strategy have dropped fewer counts (i.e. engaged in less charge bargaining) when filing
superseding indictments (see Appendix 5). It should also be noted that the higher conviction rate
for political persecution is not due to count severity either. As Appendix 6 shows, the average
count severity faced by each of the defense strategies is approximately the same.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
87
A traditional defense strategy was used by more than 50 percent of the defendants in this
study. Those defendants were convicted at a higher rate (83.2%) than were defendants who used
a disassociation defense (80.7%), but they did much better than defendants who relied on a
political persecution strategy (88.3%). Additionally, prosecutors dropped an average of .77
counts from each indictment via charge bargaining. While this is a much lower average number
of counts dropped than when defendants used the disassociation defense, it is over 4 times higher
than defendants who used the political persecution defense.
3. THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN PROSECUTORIAL AND DEFENSE STRATEGIES
In Hypothesis 1, we predicted that a conventional criminality prosecution strategy would
produce the highest conviction rates in those cases where defendants used a traditional defense
strategy. The findings indicate that to be only partially correct. While the combination of
conventional criminality and traditional defense did produce a conviction rate of 92.1 percent,
conventional criminality and political innuendo each produced higher conviction rates when
defendants used a political persecution strategy. When analyzed by regression model, the
political persecution defense strategy slightly increased the odds of conviction compared to a
traditional defense—a result we had not foreseen. The trend was the same regardless of
prosecution method.
One of the cases in which defendants used a political persecution strategy to win
acquittals provides one possible explanation for why defendants have relied on this defense. It
also provides some insight into why the defense fails more often than other defense strategies. In
U.S. v Maguire, discussed above, the defendants successfully introduced evidence of the political
and religious persecution allegedly committed by the British Government after the prosecutors
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
88
politicized the defendants’ connection to the Irish Republican Army. At the trial’s conclusion,
each defendant was acquitted.
From studying the documents in the case file, it is not possible to know exactly why the
jury returned acquittals. What is known, though, is how volatile the prosecution felt the
information would be to a jury. Prosecutors filed dozens of motions attempting to block the
defendants’ evidence, citing fears that it would inflame the jury. Smith and Damphousse have
argued that terrorism defendants might go to trial more often than non-terrorist defendants
because trials provide a stage from which terrorists can raise awareness of, and generate
sympathy for, their cause. For an ideologically committed defendant indicted in a politicized
case, the potential of having a jury decide the defendant’s actions were justified is probably quite
attractive.
As mentioned above, political persecution is a strategy and not a recognized affirmative
defense.
24
Defense attorneys are aware of available defenses and strategies and are likely to
advise their client of those options. But if the goal of an ideologically committed defendant is
vindication, he or she might feel the political persecution strategy is worth the risk. The low
success rate for most defendants who use this defense suggests that juries are not receptive to
claims of political persecution by the U.S. Government. Moreover, the findings in this study
indicate that juries may actually penalize defendants who assert those claims. Here is where the
Maguire case is different: the alleged government oppression was being conduced by a foreign
government. That might have made the argument easier to accept for the Maguire jury.
Political innuendo was another prosecution strategy that produced a high conviction rate
(96.7%) in cases where the defendant used a political persecution strategy. Because the alleged
24
Although, a defendant using a political persecution strategy might employ any number of affirmative defenses,
such as duress, necessity, entrapment, and etc.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
89
connection between a defendant and terrorism is little more than a suggestion in political
innuendo cases, using a highly politicized defense probably increases the likelihood of
conviction in two ways. First, FRE 403 limits the amount of prejudicial evidence that may
introduced by prosecutors. In cases where charges are not related to terrorism, prosecutors may
be limited to using subtle hints. When defendants claim that they are the subject of political
persecution, they may inadvertently bolster the prosecution’s claims.
Second, political innuendo cases involve traditional criminal violations in which guilt is
proved irrespective of a defendant’s political motivations. Sometimes, those counts are strict-
liability offenses. Because political motivation is irrelevant in these cases, there is very little
basis for defendants to claim they are being prosecuted due their political ideologies or because
of group memberships. Indeed, the findings in table 4.02 suggest that using a political
persecution defense strategy to counter a political innuendo prosecution strategy results in a
much higher conviction rate than the overall model average.
25
The combination of conventional criminality with both political persecution and
disassociation defense strategies produced 100 percent conviction rates, but the number of cases
was very small. Political persecution was used to counter a conventional criminality prosecution
strategy by 5 defendants, and the disassociation defense was used by just 2 defendants. The
number of cases is too small to test the relationships statistically. However, in the case of
political persecution, it is likely that defendants raise the same issues in conventional criminality
cases that defendants raised in political innuendo cases.
Defendants who used a traditional defense method to counter a political innuendo
prosecution method were convicted 4.1 percent more often than the overall model conviction rate
25
A binary logistic regression model with political innuendo and political persecution was not significant (.12 and
.09, respectively), probably due to the small number of case (n=30).
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
90
(Table 4.01), representing the final combination of strategies to produce convictions above the
model average. However, the logistic regression model in Table 4.04 indicated a traditional
defense method, compared to political persecution defense, lowered the odds of conviction. The
results indicate that political innuendo is not a significant predictor of the likelihood of
conviction. But as discussed above, it probably has a strong impact on whether a defendant is
released on bail, and that in turn, may have an impact on the likelihood of conviction.
In hypothesis 2, we predicted that an explicit politicality prosecution strategy would
produce the lowest conviction rate in cases where the defendants relied on the disassociation
strategy. As with the first hypothesis, the results only partially support that prediction. The
results showed that, overall, explicit politicality was used by prosecutors with the least success,
producing a conviction rate of just 77.4 percent. Likewise, the results showed that the
disassociation defense produced the lowest conviction rate of all defense methods, at just 80.7
percent. However, the combination produced only the second lowest conviction rate. The
combination of a traditional defense and explicit politicality resulted in a model-low conviction
rate of 71.3 percent (n=115).
Those results indicate that defendants who attempt to disassociate from their group or
ideology are generally more successful than those who claim to be the subject of political
persecution, but ignoring the political nature of the prosecutor’s case and focusing on traditional
defense methods is the most successful course of action. Given the lower conviction rate when a
traditional defense strategy was used, there are a few possible explanations concerning the effect
of disassociation. First, explicit politicality is affecting the outcomes and disassociation might
have no effect, or actually increase the odds of conviction. Second, disassociation does lower the
potential for conviction, but it does not lower the probability as strongly as a traditional defense.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
91
Results of the logistic regression help answer the question. In Table 4.03, we saw that, compared
to traditional defense, disassociation increased the odds of conviction. An in Table 4.04,
disassociation did not alter the chances of conviction compared to the political persecution
defense strategy.
Those results also show that using an explicit politicality prosecution strategy has a
negative effect on conviction rates. In the logistic regression analysis, explicit politicality
increased the odds of conviction compared to the conventional criminality prosecution methods.
Conversely, using a politicized defense strategy had the opposite effect. Defendants who used
political persecution were convicted at a higher rate than those who used other defense strategies.
Logistic regression indicated that the political persecution defense strategy increased the odds of
conviction compared to a traditional defense. Political persecution defense strategy and explicit
politicality combined to produce a slightly lower conviction rate than the overall model
conviction rate, and that would be consistent with the regression results. When considering that
the overall success rate for explicit politicality was low (77.4%), it appears that using a political
persecution strategy improved the conviction rate for that prosecution strategy. Unfortunately,
the number of cases in the samples was too small to permit testing the interaction effect between
political persecution defense strategy and explicit politicality.
Looking at the results of cases where the disassociation strategy was used to counter
political innuendo, and comparing those cases were a traditional defense was used, another
interesting fact emerges. Disassociation produced lower conviction rates than a traditional
defense. Even though disassociation was the most successful when used to counter explicit
politicality, defendants who employed a traditional defense fared better. But when compared to
other defense strategies, disassociation was the most successful in political innuendo cases. The
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
92
results suggest that the difference between the type and amount of evidence offered by the
prosecution regarding a defendant’s motive, group membership, or ideology may be the key.
Recall that with the political innuendo strategy the prosecution attempts to link the
defendant to terrorism in cases where the defendant is charged with non-politicized criminal
violations. In some instances the prosecution provides a subtle hint, but in others the prosecution
blatantly makes the claim. Importantly, the prosecution’s references to terrorism typically end
there. By contrast, in explicit politicality cases the prosecution has introduced the defendant’s
motive as a question of fact, so evidence of a defendant’s involvement with a group may be
offered. In a case where the defendant’s involvement with a group or ideology is an element of
the charges, the prosecution has a much greater chance of being permitted by the court to use
evidence of affiliation/ideology. In political innuendo cases, FRE 403 can be used to block
evidence that is prejudicial to the defendant if it is not materially related to the charges. That
means in political innuendo cases a defendant has a stronger argument to prevent the
introduction of terrorist group affiliation. It might also be the case that jurors are sympathetic to
the disassociation strategy in cases where the prosecution makes the allegation but is unable to
offer supporting evidence.
4.
SUMMARY
In this chapter, the relationships between the different prosecutorial and defense
strategies used in federal terrorism case were presented. The conventional criminality
prosecution strategy proved to be the most successful strategy overall, and it produced the
highest conviction rates, albeit only in a handful of cases where defendants used either the
political innuendo or dissociation defense strategies. However, the political innuendo
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
93
prosecution strategy, when used in cases relying on the political persecution defense produced
the highest, statistically reliable results. The results showed that this outcome was most likely
due to the political persecution defense strategy, which had a positive effect on the probability of
conviction.
Among all prosecution strategies, explicit politicality produced the lowest proportion of
successful prosecutions (convictions), as did the disassociation defense strategy. When
compared to the overall model average, the two strategies resulted in a lower proportion of
convictions compared to other strategies. Explicit politicality and a traditional defense strategy,
on the other han, was the combination that resulted in the lowest proportion of convictions. That
trend did not continue across the different combinations. The findings showed that the
disassociation defense strategy was more successful than the traditional defense strategy when
used against the political innuendo prosecution strategy. Most likely this result occurred because
of the nature of political innuendo cases and the amount and type of evidence that are used to
link defendants to terrorism.
The results indicate that using the explicit politicality prosecution strategy presents
prosecutors with the biggest challenge for gaining convictions. The findings produced a
statistically significant negative effect on the likelihood of conviction even when the impact of
evidentiary strength, case complexity, and count severity are controlled. Likewise, defendants
who use the political persecution strategy, regardless of prosecution method, are statistically
more likely to be convicted than when using an alternative defense strategy. But as discussed,
that may be situational, as it appears easier for defendants to convince a jury that they are the
victims of political persecution when there is a foreign government involved.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
94
VI: HOW 9/11 CHANGED PROSECUTORIAL AND DEFENSE STRATEGIES USED IN
FEDERAL TERRORISM CASES
In this chapter, the discussion centers on the findings for research question two - an
analysis of how 9/11 impacted the federal government’s response to terrorism. The first three
sections focus on the different categories of case type and their relationship to prosecution
strategies, defense strategies, and how those strategies affect case outcomes. Figure 6a provides
a diagram of the relationships that will be discussed.
Figure 6a
Organization Chart
Case Types Prosecution Methods Defense Methods
Outcomes
The first section contains a brief description of the three case type categories. The second
section focuses on the policy changes that occurred in the aftermath of 9/11 and provides an
explanation for why pretextual and event-linked cases were analyzed separately from diffusion
cases. Attention in the next section turns to the explicit politicality prosecution strategy and its
relationship with pretextual and event-linked cases. That is followed by a section on the political
Diffusion
Pretextual
Event-linked
Conventional
Political Innuendo
Explicit Politicality
Traditional
Political Persecution
Disassociation
Trial Conviction
Guilty Plea
Acquittal
Dismissal
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
95
innuendo strategy in pretextual and event-linked cases. Then, concentration shifts in the fifth
section to the relationship between defense strategies and case type. After a discussion of
diffusion cases in the sixth section, the seventh section provides a discussion of the findings
obtained from hypothesis testing in Chapter 4. The chapter concludes with a discussion of
theoretical implications.
1.
CASE TYPE
Recall that in Chapter 2, two separate critiques have challenged the FBI’s claims of
success in prosecuting defendants linked to terrorism since 9/11. The “data reliability” critique
questioned the data used by the FBI because some of the cases listed in FBI annual reports
contained defendants with no link to a terrorist ideology/group and those defendants were
charged with violations of law that did not appear to be related to terrorism. The “soft sentence”
critique focused on the defendants’ prison sentence lengths, reporting that the war on terror was
not as successful as the DOJ claimed because “terrorists” were receiving sentences of just a few
months. Chesney (2007) addressed both critiques, arguing that one must make a distinction
between the different types of terrorism cases in order to evaluate the effectiveness of
antiterrorism policy.
Chesney appears to be partially correct, as case types are indeed important. But to
evaluate the effectiveness of antiterrorism policy, one must first understand the post-9/11
changes in antiterrorism policy and identify the new policy goals. Shifting away from the goal
of infiltrating terror groups, Attorney General Ashcroft directed the FBI and the EOUSA to
intercept and disrupt terrorist planning before an act of terrorism could occur. While developing
his case type categories, Chesney mentions that the FBI and EOUSA targeted immigration and
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
96
financial fraud after 9/11. These cases involved defendants who were neither linked to a terrorist
group/ideology nor an act of terrorism by the FBI. He called those cases “diffusion prevention”,
because making it difficult for anyone to engage in immigration and financial fraud would make
it difficult for terrorists, who relied on both forms of criminality, to succeed in gathering the
resources and personnel necessary to carry out future attacks. Those cases are at the center of the
above mentioned critiques.
Chesney provided the basic framework for establishing case type categories, but goes no
further. After describing what diffusion cases were, Chesney turned the remainder of his
attention to cases involving 18 USC §2332, the statute making it unlawful to provide material
support to terrorist groups. Conversely, this study focuses on everything but material support
cases (see Figure 6b for description of case type and prosecution method categories). Building
on Chesney’s description, we defined and analyzed case type in Chapter 4. The following case
type categories were used: the event-linked category was comprised of cases where the
defendant was linked to a terrorist group or ideology and was indicted on charges related to an
act of terrorism (planned or completed); the pretextual category contained cases where the
defendant was linked to a terrorist group or ideology, but was charged with crimes not directly
related to an act of terrorism, and; the diffusion category contained cases where the defendant
was neither linked to a group or ideology, nor a crime related to an act of terrorism (see Chapter
3 for coding).
Figure 6b
Case Type and Prosecution Method Reference Chart
Diffusion Case Pretextual Case Event-linked Case
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
97
Conventional
Criminality
Prosecution Method
Not linked to Act of
terrorism in case
documents
No hint or accusation of
ideology by prosecutor in
case.
No evidence of group/
ideology after FBI
investigation/before
indictment
Charged with non-
politicized criminal counts
Not linked to Act of
terrorism in case
documents
No hint or accusation of
group/ideology by
prosecutor in case.
* FBI linked Defendant to
Group/Ideology
Charged with non-
politicized criminal counts
Linked to Act of terrorism
in case documents (actual
or planned)
No hint or accusation of
group/ideology by
prosecutor in case.
FBI linked Defendant to
Group/Ideology
Charged with non-
politicized criminal counts
Political
Innuendo
Prosecution Method
Not linked to Act of
terrorism in case
documents
Hint or accusation of group
or ideology in case.
No evidence of group or
ideology after FBI
investigation/before
indictment
Charged with non-
politicized criminal counts
Not linked to Act of
terrorism in case
documents
Hint or accusation of group
or ideology in case
FBI linked Defendant to
Group/Ideology
Charged with non-
politicized criminal counts
Linked to Act of terrorism
in case documents (actual
or planned)
Hint or accusation of group
or ideology in case
FBI linked Defendant to
Group/Ideology
Charged with non-
politicized criminal counts
Explicit
Politicality
Prosecution Method
Not linked to Act of
terrorism in case
documents
**(Circumstantial)
Evidence of group or
ideology presented in case
No evidence of group or
ideology after FBI
investigation/before
indictment
Charged with conspiracy or
similar counts, implicates
motive
Not linked to Act of
terrorism in case
documents
Evidence of group or
ideology presented in case
FBI linked Defendant to
Group/Ideology
Charged with conspiracy or
similar counts, implicates
motive
Linked to Act of terrorism
in case documents (actual
or planned)
Evidence of group or
ideology presented in case
FBI linked Defendant to
Group/Ideology
Charged with conspiracy or
similar counts, implicates
motive
* Information derived from all FBI case lists created prior to August 2002, case documents and press releases on
cases filed after September 2002.
** We use the term “circumstantial” here because there is typically no concrete evidence of the defendant’s
membership in a group or his/her ties to an extremist ideology. Nonetheless, prosecutors have attempted to get
juries to draw a connection between these defendants and terrorism based on other factors.
2. PRETEXTUAL AND EVENT-LINKED CASES
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
98
The analyses conducted in Chapter 4 demonstrate the effects of the antiterrorism policy
shift in 2001. There were no diffusion cases listed by the FBI prior to 9/11, yet this category
represents almost half of the cases filed afterwards (see Table 4.03). Taking the diffusion cases
out of the post-9/11 sample is important because the resulting mix of
event-linked and pretextual cases closely resembles the composition of cases filed prior to 9/11.
This allows a comparison of similar cases before and after 9/11 while placing diffusion cases in a
category to be studied separately.
Before 9/11, 85 percent of all terrorism cases were event-linked, while only 15 percent
were pretextual. After 9/11, when diffusion cases were removed, only 57 percent of the
terrorism cases were event-linked, while the percentage of pretextual cases increased to 43
percent. This is to be expected. The policy shift ushered in by Attorney General Ashcroft
refocused law enforcement efforts towards intercepting and interrupting terrorist groups before
those groups could successfully plan an attack. By charging potential terrorists as soon as
criminal violations occurred, logically, there would be less evidence available for prosecutors to
link defendants with terrorist acts.
The average number of defendants indicted, per year, in event-linked cases was slightly
lower in the post-9/11 era, decreasing from approximately 22 to 17 annually.
26
The average
number of defendants indicted in pretextual cases increased from 4 to 13 annually.
In this study, the total number of defendants who were indicted in cases that the FBI
linked to a terrorist ideology (total of event-linked and pretextual cases) increased slightly from
26 to 30 per year.
27
These trends fit within the theoretical framework of the hydraulic effect. The
26
It should be reiterated that there are more terrorism cases to be collected from the 2002 to 2004 FBI list (ATS and
PADS). Anecdotally, the proportion of pretextual vis-à-vis event-linked cases should not change dramatically.
27
The total number of event-linked and pretextual cases in the post-9/11 era will increase as the remaining cases are
collected and added to the database, and, anecdotally speaking, the total number of defendants indicted in event-
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
99
hydraulic effect predicts that get-tough policy changes have very little impact on the crimes they
target because the government’s resources are already directed towards stopping the most serious
forms of crime. Here, the more serious forms of terrorism crime would be terrorism attacks—
event-linked cases. According to the hydraulic effect, law enforcement officials and prosecutors
have no choice but to target less serious crimes. Even in the absence of diffusion cases, that
trend is present in the form of an increased number of pretextual cases.
3. EXPLICIT POLITICALITY IN PRETEXTUAL AND EVENT-LINKED CASES
After 9/11 prosecutors relied on the explicit politicality prosecution method substantially
less than before 9/11. Overall, the percentage of cases prosecuted with this method dropped
from 66.1 percent to 25.3 percent. Even when diffusion cases were removed, prosecutors used
explicit politicality 48 percent of the time. Among event-linked cases, the use of explicit
politicality dropped from 70.9 percent of the cases to 60 percent in the post-9/11era. In
pretextual cases, prosecutors used explicit politicality method only 27 percent of the time
compared 40.7 percent of the time before 9/11.
This change could be the result of prosecutorial choice, but it may have been a direct
result of the shift towards prosecuting defendants sooner rather than later. Recall that average
count severity did not experience a similar decrease between eras. That means that defendants in
the post-9/11 era were being charged with counts of similar severity, but prosecutors were not
explicitly politicizing the cases. It is likely that as cases were referred to prosecutors sooner, the
FBI and other law enforcement personnel were providing prosecutors with less evidence that
prosecutors might use in the more highly politicized prosecution method.
linked cases after 9/11 will remain close to the average for the pre-9/11 era. Similarly, we expect the average
number of defendants indicted for pretextual cases to climb slightly higher than the figures reported.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
100
In explicit politicality cases, prosecutors typically charge defendants with counts that
make motive a component of the government’s case. As Smith and Damphousse (1998) have
pointed out, prosecutors in America have historically avoided raising motive. It may be the case
that prosecutors in the post-9/11 era have enough evidence to prosecute terrorist defendants for
crimes just as severe as before 9/11, but have not had enough evidence to use explicit politicality.
If this is what has happened, it could be viewed as an unintended benefit. The use of explicit
politicality decreases the odds of a conviction, so prosecuting defendants earlier might help
explain the higher conviction rate in the post-9/11 era.
4. POLITICAL INNUENDO IN PRETEXTUAL AND EVENT-LINKED CASES
Before 9/11, prosecutors used political innuendo just 22.7 percent of the time. After 9/11
prosecutors used the strategy 22.3 percent of the time overall. However, removing diffusion
cases revealed a much greater reliance: political innuendo was used against 35 percent of all
defendants indicted in pretextual and event-linked cases. There was an increase of 6.5 percent in
event-linked cases, representing a total of 30 percent of all defendants indicted for such cases
after 9/11.
Pretextual cases saw the largest increase in the use of the political innuendo prosecution
strategy. Before 9/11 prosecutors used political innuendo against only 18.5 percent of the
defendants charged in pretextual cases. That total increased by 24.6 percent to a rate of 43.1
percent of all defendants in the post-9/11 era. This lends support to the possibility that
prosecutors were beginning criminal cases sooner and without the evidence necessary to pursue
an explicit politicality prosecution strategy. Recall that FRE 403 requires that the amount of
evidence necessary to pursue explicit politicality strategy, and thereby connect a defendant to an
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
101
act (or planned act) of terrorism, is much higher than the evidence needed to use the political
innuendo strategy.
Like the results in event-linked cases, prosecutors who filed pretextual cases enjoyed
higher conviction rates in the post-9/11 era than before. Defendants who were indicted in
pretextual cases before 9/11 were convicted 87 percent of the time. The conviction rate for
defendants indicted in pretextual cases after 9/11 increased to 92.6 percent. While the
percentage of defendants who were convicted at trial remained about the same (see Tables 4.05
and 4.06), the rate of defendants who entered guilty pleas increased from 75.4 percent to 81.5
percent. Similarly, the dismissal rate decreased by from 13.0 percent to 7.4 percent (there were
no acquittals in either era). Those numbers are consistent with the trends reported by
Damphousse and Shields (2007), who found that defendants who were indicted after a major
terrorism event were more likely to plead guilty than to take their chances with a jury.
5. DEFENSE STRATEGIES IN PRETEXTUAL AND EVENT-LINKED CASES
The higher conviction rates for pretextual cases and event-linked cases in the post-9/11
era may have also occurred, in part, because of defense tactics: namely, a shift away from the
disassociation defense strategy to the traditional defense strategy used by defendants indicted in
event-linked cases, and; a shift from the disassociation strategy to the political persecution
strategy by defendants indicted in pretextual cases. With regard to the first relationship, our
findings provide evidence that defendants who relied on a traditional defense strategy were 5
percent more likely to be convicted than defendants who relied on a disassociation strategy in
political innuendo cases. As mentioned above, there was an increase in the use of the political
innuendo prosecution strategy in event-linked cases. Defendants charged in event-linked cases
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
102
used a disassociation defense 10.3 percent less often in the post-9/11 era, while they used the
traditional defense 13.5 percent more often. In other words, while prosecutors began using a
more successful prosecution strategy, defendants relied more heavily on a less successful defense
strategy.
Regarding the second relationship, recall that the least successful defense strategy, in
terms of winning acquittals and dismissals, was the political persecution strategy—defendants
who relied on this strategy increased their likelihood of being convicted (see Table 4.03). In the
post-9/11 era, there was a 27.7 percent increase in the proportion of pretextual cases filed (after
diffusion cases were removed from the sample). Of those defendants charged in pretextual
cases, there was a 26.1 percent increase in the proportion of defendants who relied on the
political persecution defense strategy, and a 21.8 percent decrease in the proportion of
defendants who relied on disassociation. Meanwhile, prosecutors relied on the political
innuendo prosecution strategy 24.6 percent more often. In other words, while prosecutors tried
more pretextual cases using the political innuendo prosecution strategy in the post-9/11 era,
defendants shifted from the most successful defense strategy (disassociation) to the least
successful defense strategy (political persecution) in large numbers.
6.
DIFFUSION CASES
In the post-9/11 era, 79 defendants were indicted in diffusion cases. As expected with
cases where the FBI did not link the defendant to a completed or planned act of terrorism,
prosecutors relied almost exclusively on the conventional criminality prosecution strategy.
Unlike in other case types, only a few prosecutors attempted to use politicized strategies in
diffusion cases. Six defendants were tried using the political innuendo strategy, and 2 defendants
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
103
were tried using the explicit politicality strategy. The overall conviction rate for diffusion cases
was 94.8 percent—the highest of all case types.
The high conviction rate is predictable, given that 91.9 percent of the defendants relied on
traditional defense strategies. When combined with conventional criminality, this defense
strategy produced an overall conviction rate of 92.5 percent (see Table 4.01). Recall that when
conventional criminality was combined with either political persecution or disassociation, it
produced 100 percent conviction rates. The high conviction rates for this particular case type are
similar to overall federal conviction rates (see discussion in Chapter 2) because diffusion cases
are most similar to traditional cases in that the defendants are not typically linked to terrorism by
the FBI. The guilty plea rate in diffusion cases is 88.3 percent. In fact, only 6 defendants
indicted in diffusion cases took their cases to trial.
Both structural contextual theory and the hydraulic effect can help explain the number of
diffusion cases in the post-9/11 era despite the lack of them in the pre-9/11 era. First, structural
contextual theory is based on the premise that when some forms of crime are perceived as a
problem, public pressure forces components of the criminal justice system to focus their attention
on that criminality. The criminal justice system, which is normally composed of loosely coupled
parts that compete for resources, begins to operate more cohesively. As applied to terrorism
cases (see Smith and Damphousse, 1998, Damphousse and Shields, 2007), that increased
cohesion sometimes leads to net widening, or aggressively pursuing offenders.
Diffusion cases can be seen, partially, as a product of net widening that occurred as a
direct result of 9/11. The events of 9/11 raised the national consciousness of terrorism attacks to
a record level (Silverlieb, 2008). Those events also resulted in the government making an
enormous commitment of resources to fight terrorism. The events of 9/11 also led to increased
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
104
efforts to improve collaboration between the many investigative components of the federal and
state governments. One result of these changes was an increase in the number of defendants the
DOJ claimed to have prosecuted. The number of defendants listed by the FBI as having been
indicted in terrorism-related cases in the three years following 9/11 was larger than the combined
number of terrorist defendants indicted during the previous 20 years (Damphousse and Shields,
2007).
However, as the above findings show, the average number of terrorism defendants
indicted in event-linked cases did not increase when compared to the previous 20 years. The
dramatic increase occurred through prosecuting defendants in diffusion cases. Again, to diffuse
possible acts of terrorism, diffusion cases were filed against defendants who engaged in
criminality that the government viewed as a prerequisite for engaging in terrorism. However, the
government’s pursuit of diffusion cases was not merely the result of “tightened coupling,” it was
also the product of specific policy changes.
The hydraulic effect suggests that get-tough policy changes directed at a given form of
criminality will not impact the target criminality as substantially as it will less serious, but related
types of criminal behavior (see Chapter 2 discussion). Damphousse and Shields (2007) used the
hydraulic effect to describe the impact that get-tough policy changes, enacted after the Oklahoma
City bombing in 1995,
28
had on the way the government prosecuted terrorism cases. They found
that prosecutors engaged in net widening by charging defendants with violations of law that
prosecutors had seldom used, or simply ignored before the bombing.
Unlike that 2007 study, the focus here is not necessarily what charges federal prosecutors
used before and after 9/11, but the change in the U.S. Attorney General’s guidelines that led
prosecutors to begin pursuing diffusion cases. When Attorney General Ashcroft directed the FBI
28
While Damphousse and Shields did analyze post-9/11 cases in that 2007 study, they did so with a small sample.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
105
and EOUSA to proactively prosecute defendants with the purpose of interrupting future acts of
terrorism before they could occur, prosecutors probably had little choice but to cast a wider net.
As noted above, even with increased manpower provided to the FBI and other law enforcement
agencies, and broader legal authority for them to engage in surveillance, the average number of
event-linked cases did not increase. Even the average number of pretextual cases saw only a
slight increase. As a result, prosecutors devoted more attention to diffusing potential acts of
terrorism by focusing on immigration and financial fraud.
7. CASE CHARACTERISTICS BEFORE AND AFTER 9/11
Focus now changes from case type and legal strategies to specific changes in case
characteristics. Seven hypotheses, based on structural contextual theory and the hydraulic effect,
were tested in Chapter 4. Hypothesis 3 predicted that if tightened coupling had occurred among
components of the criminal justice system in the post-9/11 era, prosecutors would voluntarily
dismiss fewer counts than they did in the pre-9/11 era. When diffusion cases were included,
results supported the hypothesis. However, when diffusion cases were removed, results showed
an increase in the number cases dismissed. Similarly, Hypothesis 4 predicted that tightened
coupling would result in lower plea bargain rates among pretextual cases in the post-9/11 era.
The opposite occurred.
These hypotheses were based on findings in previous studies (e.g. Smith and
Damphousse, 1998) that found that tightened coupling occurred differently in terrorism cases
than Hagan’s drug cases. In particular, plea bargain rates were dramatically lower among
terrorism cases than in other federal criminal cases. Hagan found that tightened coupling in drug
cases increased the likelihood that prosecutors would engage in plea bargains in order to ensure
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
106
convictions. Why then did terrorist defendants demand trials as such a high rate before 9/11, and
why did that change afterwards?
This study provides some likely answers. Terrorism cases, especially event-linked, are as
much a challenge to a defendant’s political (and frequently religious) beliefs as they are an
indictment of his/her criminal behavior. Unlike drug cases, which typically focus on the
defendant’s criminal behavior only, pleading guilty in terrorism cases means not only “doing
time,” but it also means that one gives up fighting for his/her political (and/or religious) beliefs.
Before 9/11, terrorism cases were comprised only of pretextual and event-linked cases:
most of those cases were the latter. Prosecutors also relied on the explicit politicality prosecution
strategy in the majority of cases. Even if prosecutors had aggressively sought plea bargains
among terrorist defendants, the type of cases they filed and the prosecution strategies they used
lowered the chances of plea bargaining. In the post-9/11 era, much of that changed. The
Ashcroft Guidelines affected that change in two ways.
First, requiring the FBI and EOUSA to intercept and prosecute cases sooner likely
lowered the amount of evidence necessary to pursue event-linked cases. Second, the lower
amount of evidence may be responsible for the dramatic reduction in the percentage of cases in
which prosecutors used an explicit politicality prosecution strategy. So it is possible that the
Ashcroft Guidelines inadvertently made it easier for prosecutors to secure guilty pleas by taking
some of the politics (and religion) out of terrorism cases. As noted above, the more that
terrorism cases resemble traditional criminal cases, the more likely that terrorism defendants will
behave like traditional criminal defendants. Couple that with the likelihood that 9/11 left
terrorist defendants less willing to take a chance with a jury, and higher plea rates make sense.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
107
The increased number of dismissed cases may also be related to the Ashcroft Guidelines.
Even though the rate of guilty pleas increased and the number of acquittals decreased, the
number of cases dismissed went up in the post-9/11 era (when diffusion cases were removed).
Moreover, the average number of counts dismissed per case doubled (these figures do not
include counts dismissed due to plea bargaining, see Table 4.11). There are at least two reasons
for this. First, the increase in the number of cases dismissed may be the direct result of
prosecutors having less evidence. Second, it is possible that net widening resulted in prosecutors
charging defendants with extra counts while planning to dismiss some in order to negotiate a
guilty plea.
Hypothesis 5 tested the hydraulic effect and predicted that get-tough policy changes in
the post-9/11 era would cause prosecutors to cast a wider net and charge defendants with less
severe crimes than in the pre-9/11 era. This was clearly the case when diffusion cases were
included in the analysis. However, when diffusion cases were removed, there was no
statistically significant change.
Compared to event-linked cases, Hypothesis 6 predicted that net widening would result in
a higher proportion of pretextual cases being filed in the post-9/11 era. The proportion of
defendants indicted in pretextual cases nearly tripled in the post-9/11 era. Taken together, the
tests of Hypotheses 5 and 6 demonstrate that prosecutors not only pursued less serious charges
(via diffusion cases) but they also relied on less politicized prosecution strategies (across all case
types). Both are likely results of the Ashcroft Guidelines and federal prosecutors desire to
appear more proactive to the general public. Therefore, both results are consistent with the net
widening components of the hydraulic effect and structural contextual theory.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
108
Hypothesis 7 predicted that policy changes implemented after 9/11 would result in a
smaller average number of defendants indicted in event-linked cases. In fact, the results showed
that the average number of defendants indicted in event-linked cases dropped by half, to less than
two people per case. Even though the Ashcroft Guidelines continued the trend of the preceding
guidelines and defined terrorism as a group activity, the number of individual defendants, even in
event-linked cases, grew. One explanation is found elsewhere in the Ashcroft Guidelines.
Recall the discussion from Chapter 2, which detailed the process the FBI followed to
open domestic security/terrorism enterprise investigations prior to 2002. FBI field agents were
required to get permission from the FBI headquarters before a terrorism investigation could be
opened. The Ashcroft Guidelines placed complete authority for opening those investigations
into the hands of field agents. Remember also that a terrorism investigation could be conducted
over a much longer timeframe than a general crimes investigation, and terrorism investigations
also provided legal authority via the USA PATRIOT Act (among others) for field agents to use
more invasive techniques (e.g. roving wire taps, secret records checks, etc.). Consistent with net
widening, the combination of the new guidelines and the Attorney General’s mandate to
prosecute cases sooner led to the FBI referring cases to prosecutors that contained fewer
defendants.
Similarly, Hypothesis 8 predicted that the proportion of event-linked cases involving
informants would be lower in the post-9/11 era. It was suspected that the “early prosecution
mandate demanded by Attorney General Ashcroft would impede the government’s ability to
infiltrate extremists groups with agents, and likewise, it would limit the amount of time
government agents had to develop relationships with potential informants who were associated
with group members. The hypothesis was tested using a number of models evaluating the
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
109
number of confidential informants, the number of undercover agents, and the average level of
assistance per case that each provided.
All of the findings supported the hypothesis: Attorney General Ashcroft’s mandate had a
strong negative impact on the number of informants who provided evidence for the government.
Similarly, the shift nearly eliminated the government’s ability to infiltrate terrorist groups with
undercover agents. Finally, even when informants could be secured, the amount of evidence
they provided was lower than in the pre-9/11 era. These findings support other points discussed
above: namely, that the policy shift may have been responsible for limiting the amount of
evidence available to prosecutors. Lower amounts of evidence probably caused a shift in the
type of cases prosecutors pursued and the type of prosecution strategies they employed. Finally,
lower levels of evidence might be the cause of the increase in the number of case dismissals.
Hypothesis 9 predicted that conviction rates on more serious charges would go down
after 9/11. The results did not support the hypothesis. In fact, conviction rates among high
severity charges increased significantly in the post-9/11 era. Moreover, there was no statistical
difference between high and low severity case in the post-9/11 era (see Table 4.21). It is
possible that the overall increase in conviction rates in the post-9/11 era is tied to marked
decrease in the use of highly politicized prosecution strategies in that era, leading to an increase
in the number of guilty pleas. The relationships between case type, prosecution strategy, defense
strategy and count severity require further study.
8.
THEORETICAL IMPLICATIONS AND NEW CONCEPTS
Many of the hypotheses used to test structural contextual theory were confirmed, while
others were not. Relying on the findings of previous terrorism studies, predictions were
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
110
formulated to explain that 9/11 was likely to lower plea bargain rates. The findings indicate that
plea bargain rates increased after 9/11, and that, in turn, provided a possible explanation for why
plea bargain rates in terrorism cases filed prior to 9/11 were dramatically different than plea
bargain rates in non-terrorism criminal cases.
As the findings in this chapter suggest, when prosecutors introduced politicality into
terrorism cases, they increased the likelihood that the defendant would demand a jury trial. We
recognize that causality may well have worked in the opposite direction - that a defendant who
demanded a jury trial would result in the prosecutor having to politicize the trial to make the
conviction more probable. That said, the timing of the politicalization process suggests
otherwise. In most cases, for example, the introduction of politicality was made in the
indictment itself or during some other pre-trial stage of the process. The defendant’s decision to
go to trial was not made until after the trial had become politicized. Thus, logic suggests that the
decision by the prosecutor to express more politicality in the case affected how the defendant
acted (or “reacted”). During the pre-9/11 era, prosecutors relied on politicized prosecution
strategies a majority of the time. For the reasons discussed at length above, after 9/11
prosecutors relied less on politicized strategies. In turn, plea bargain rates and conviction rates
increased. We refer to this as the ideology effect. The ideology effect can be explained as
follows: as prosecutors increase the level of politicality in a case, they increase the
contentiousness of the case as defendants become more defensive of their beliefs and values. As
the contentiousness increases, so to do the chances a case will result in a trial.
The implications of our findings suggest an interesting interpretation of the relationship
between the prosecution and defense teams in terrorism cases. Prosecutors increase the
politicality of a case when they increase the focus of the case on a defendant’s motive and/or
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
111
ideology in addition to the facts of the case. In the terrorism cases we have studied, increased
politicality may be drawing into question not only a defendant’s culpability with regard to law
breaking, but also his/her ideological belief system. It may be that the more that a terrorist’s
ideological belief system is questioned, the less likely that he/she will enter a guilty plea. It is a
simple matter of cognitive dissonance. A guilty plea in a non-politicized case merely requires a
defendant to admit culpability for illicit behavior in exchange for a lower sentence. A guilty plea
in a politicized case not only includes an admission of culpability, it also requires the defendant
to stop fighting for his/her beliefs. Some defendants may see a guilty plea as a tacit agreement
that they agree with the government.
The ideology effect may impact jury decisions in two ways. First, prosecutors who relied
on explicitly politicality suffered the highest number of acquittals. Second, defendants who
employed the political persecution strategy suffered the highest conviction rates. Interestingly,
defendants who relied on the disassociation strategy (an anti-politicality strategy, if you will) in
political innuendo cases were awarded acquittals and dismissals in very high numbers. It is
possible that juries react negatively to politicizing cases, regardless of which party is responsible.
While the relationship between politicality and acquittal rates was not tested in this study beyond
running crosstabs, this is an interesting area for future research.
The ideology effect provides a better understanding of how plea bargain rates may be
affected in the future. If, in long periods of time between major terrorism events, the FBI and
EOUSA return to infiltrating terrorism groups rather than interrupting them, and prosecutions
become more politicized, one would expect lower plea bargain rates, and lower conviction rates.
If, however, the focus of the FBI and EOUSA continues to be one of prosecuting defendants
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
112
early, and cases remain less politicized, one would expect plea bargain rates and, therefore,
conviction rates to remain high.
Combining structural contextual theory, the hydraulic effect and the ideology effect helps
to explain terrorism prosecution in the United States. In the absence of a major attack, terrorism
cases nonetheless received heightened scrutiny, but unlike other crimes where structural
contextual theory has been used to explain higher plea rates, the ideology effect explains why the
plea rates are low when politicality is high. The hydraulic effect helps to explain how a major
terrorism event, like 9/11, results in “get tough” policy changes. Those policy changes can bring
about net widening, which may dramatically alter how the government responds to terrorism.
After 9/11, net widening resulted in the creation of new policy goals and an entirely new case
type—diffusion cases.
9. SUMMARY
This chapter began with a discussion of the three case type categories that were first
described by Robert Chesney (2007). Chesney developed the categories in response to the soft
sentence critique and the data validitycritique that had been raised to question the DOJ’s claims
of success in its antiterrorism policy. Rather than focus on one type of criminal violation, as
Chesney did, to answer the critiques, this study focused on the policy changes that occurred in
the aftermath of 9/11. Specifically, attention was directed towards two policy changes: first, the
Attorney General’s Guidelines on opening terrorism investigations, and; secondly, Attorney
General Ashcroft’s mandate (cite needed) for the FBI and EOUSA to begin interrupting terrorist
groups by prosecuting defendants as soon as a criminal case could go forward.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
113
The discussion then turned to the analyses of the case types conducted in Chapter 4. The
results suggested that after 9/11 prosecutors relied less heavily on highly politicized prosecution
strategies and they filed fewer event-linked cases. As a result, plea bargain rates and conviction
rates increased. This, despite the finding that the average count severity in the post-9/11 era was
only slightly lower than it was in the previous era. While the decision to file less politically
charged cases could have been a conscious decision on the part of prosecutors, it was more likely
the result of less evidence: an unintended consequence of Attorney General Ashcroft’s
“prosecute early” policy. Results presented later in the Chapter, showing a dramatic drop in the
number of informants used in the post-9/11 era, provided support for that position.
Next, concentration was placed on diffusion cases. The results showed that no diffusion
cases were filed before 9/11, yet they made up the majority of cases afterwards. Cases that make
up the diffusion category are not the product of poor record keeping as some critics have
suggested. Rather, they are the product of policy changes implemented after 9/11. Attorney
General Ashcroft directed the FBI and the EOUSA to prosecute cases sooner for the purpose of
interrupting terrorists before they could complete attacks. Due to constraints caused by
prosecuting cases sooner, prosecutors began trying to diffuse potential terrorism treats by
targeting crimes that they determined were precursors to terrorism.
After the section on diffusion cases, discussion turned to hypotheses testing. While the
results supported a majority of the hypotheses that were based on structural contextual theory,
some of the hypotheses were not supported. Discussion of the unsupported hypotheses provided
a potential answer to one of the questions that has lingered in previous terrorism research: why
were guilty plea rates in terrorism cases so low in the pre-9/11 era? The results suggest that the
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
114
highly politicized prosecution strategies that dominated federal terrorism trial in the pre-9/11 era
may be the answer.
The concept of the ideology effect was introduced to explain how highly politicized cases
increase the contentiousness of terrorism trials because they implicate the belief systems of
terrorist defendants. The implication is that tightened coupling, a component of structural
contextual theory, does not operate differently in terrorism cases than it does in non-terrorism
cases (leading to lower plea bargain rates in the former while leading to higher plea bargain rates
in the latter), it is simply masked by the ideology effect. Future research on this topic is
warranted.
VII: POLICY IMPLICATIONS AND CONCLUSION
As this study began, two post-9/11 critiques of US antiterrorism policy were identified.
In both cases, the critiques were levied against the Department of Justice and its subordinate
bureaus. The critiques question the effectiveness of U.S. antiterrorism/counterterrorism policy
and the accuracy of DOJ reports, and the critiques call into question policy changes made in the
years after 9/11. After a brief summary of the policy changes mentioned in this study, this
Chapter will focus on public policy theory, and then tie theory to the GAO and OIG criticisms.
This section concludes with a discussion of policy implications as they pertain to this study. The
next section turns to a synopsis of the project which is followed by a short discussion suggesting
future research possibilities.
1.
SELECTED POLICY CHANGES SINCE 9/11
In recent years, the revised version of the Anti-Terrorism and Effective Death Penalty
Act and the USA PATRIOT Act (Patriot Act) were written to extend and strengthen U.S.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
115
antiterrorism policy. In broad terms, our antiterrorism policy is currently focused on terrorist
organizations, affiliated networks, and state sponsors in an effort to identify potential terrorist
threats and proactively prevent future attacks. In a statement released after September 11,
Attorney General Ashcroft (2002) explained that the policy of the United States government
changed from prosecuting terror-related crimes that had already occurred, to thwarting attacks
before they happen.
In the wake of September 11, Attorney General Ashcroft implemented new guidelines
that expanded the FBI’s authority to investigate domestic terrorism (Ashcroft, 2002). Section
(B)(4)(a) of the Ashcroft Guidelines loosened the prior standards by allowing field agents to
authorize a terrorism investigation for a period of up to one year—permission from the FBI
Headquarters to open investigations and renew them was no longer necessary. In addition to the
new guidelines, Attorney General Ashcroft directed the FBI and EOUSA (among other agencies)
to intercept, interrupt, and prosecute potential threats early in order to prevent attacks.
Policy implementation brought a shift of resources to the FBI. There was a dramatic
increase in the number of JTTF field offices (from 34 to 66) and the number of personnel
assigned by the FBI to its terrorism teams (from 600 to more than 7,000) (Rolince, 2003a). In
addition, DHS provided assistance to coordinate investigations between the FBI and the Bureau
of Immigration & Customs Enforcement (Rolince, 2003b). Within a few years, the DOJ began
claiming unprecedented success in fighting the war on terror. The critiques of those claims
surfaced soon afterwards.
The critiques were rooted in the difficulty associated with evaluating antiterrorism policy.
The effectiveness of domestic terrorism policy has been the subject of much controversy and
criticism, and for good reason. Policy programs hastily put in place after the 9/11 attacks lacked
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
116
any specific requirements for the FBI or EOUSA to develop mechanisms for evaluation. Those
that have been put in place since have proven problematic. As early as 2002, the General
Accountability Office (GAO) and the Office of the Inspector General (OIG) began issuing
reports critical of the DOJ (seeChapter 2).
In 2004, and at the request of Congress, the GAO completed an assessment of national
terrorism strategies (GAO-01-408T). The GAO report determined that those strategies were so
inadequately defined and implemented that the GAO could not provide an assessment on
whether they were working. Congress addressed the issue of record keeping by requiring the
DOJ to implement several internal controls (OIG-07-04). Responding to a series of negative
reports by the GAO, Congress mandated changes in internal record keeping. The requirements
were issued in the form of a mandate, a top-down approach, with Congress relying heavily on
GAO findings. In a 2007 report, the OIG found unresolved problems, stating that DOJ
components did not accurately report terrorism-related statistics in their annual budgets, financial
statements, performance plans, and statistical reports (OIG-07-04). Without accurate data, the
OIG claimed, it was not possible to evaluate whether the antiterrorism policies were effective.
The inability to evaluate a policy’s effectiveness is a hallmark of implementation failure.
In policy theory, several scholars have addressed implementation failure. Since the
policy initiatives have come in the form of legislation and directives from the Attorney General,
discussion turns to the “top-down” perspective of policy implementation. From a top-down
perspective, Sabatier and Mazmanian (1979) argue that six criteria need to be met for effective
implementation: 1) make policy objectives clear and consistent; 2) the program must be based on
a valid causal theory; 3) the implementation process must structured adequately; 4)
implementing officials must be committed to the program’s goals; 5) interest groups and
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
117
(executive and legislative) sovereigns are supportive; and 6) there are no detrimental changes in
the socioeconomic framework conditions. Mazmanian and Sabatier (1983) acknowledged that
perfect hierarchical control over the implementation process is difficult to achieve.
One of the issues facing antiterrorism policy is that there appears to be a breakdown in
establishing definitions, goals and objectives on which the different components of the
government can agree. Moreover, there is a fundamental disagreement between the FBI/EOUSA
and the GAO/OIG over the sufficiency of the terrorism-link measurement. Lipsky (1971) rejects
the idea of “hierarchical guidance.” In his view, it is impossible for Congress to draft statutes
with unequivocal policy goals and to control the implementation process from top to bottom.
Indeed, the goals and objectives of post-9/11 antiterrorism policy came from Attorney General
Ashcroft, not Congress.
Hjern (1982) argues that street level bureaucrats are the key to successful
implementation. Hjern claims that implementation occurred only when those who were
primarily affected were actively involved in the planning and execution of these programs. In
this approach, policy makers (Congress) and street level bureaucrats (FBI/EOUSA and
OIG/GOA) would be involved in policy formation. Ripley and Franklin (1982) suggest that,
even with this approach, the challenge is defining the problem in a way that satisfies each set of
stakeholders.
According to Rochefort and Cobb (1994), defining issues or problems is one of the most
crucial aspects of policymaking and also one of the most complicated. Problems find their way
onto the agenda in many different ways and are always viewed through the eyes of the beholder.
That is certainly the case here. The GAO and OIG define the problem as a lack of internal
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
118
controls (on the part of the FBI
29
and the EOUSA
30
) to accurately track cases and provide
unequivocal proof those cases are clearly related to terrorism. The lack of controls has resulted
in over- and under-estimates of the number of terrorism cases both agencies handle each year.
The FBI and EOUSA, conversely, claim the GAO and OIG ignore explanations and overestimate
the number of discrepancies.
The GAO found discrepancies between DOJ’s official statistics: namely, that the DOJ
overestimated the number of defendants in terrorism cases they processed.
31
This is an issue
repeated in each assessment of the DOJ and its components. Interestingly, these complaints are
centered on what this project calls diffusion cases. Both the OIG and the GAO have reported
that when case materials they reviewed did not support a terrorism label, they contacted the
29
We could find no information regarding the specifics of how the FBI tracks terrorism investigations. Section
(B)(4)(a) of the Ashcroft Guidelines requires that all terrorism investigations be reported to the Office of
Intelligence Policy and Review, but it makes no further demands of the FBI. The GAO report refers to a tracking
system used by the FBI called
Zeus. The specifics of that system are classified, but the GAO states that Zeus is
the primary vehicle the FBI uses to track and relay terrorism investigation information internally, to other law
enforcement agencies, and to the government. In addition, the FBI reports the number of terrorism incidents the
Agency investigates in its budget justification each year. After discovering a number of discrepancies in the
number of cases the FBI reported in 2004, the GAO asked an official with the department. The official said that
FBI relies on the originating FBI units to enter all the data in the database. The official responsible for
accumulating the statistic said that when she began her job in February 2004 there were no formalized procedures
on how to collect, verify, and report the Intelligence Assessments issued and no formalized procedures have been
developed since.
30
The EOUSA maintains a statistical monitoring system called LIONS. The EOUSA is responsible for tracking and
analyzing data related to the work of the 94 United States Attorney’s Offices (USAOs) in the development of
budget and litigation priorities. The
LIONS system is a database with on-line capabilities that permit the USAOs
and EOUSA to compile, maintain and track information relating to defendants, crimes, criminal charges, court
events, and witnesses. According to the GAO, the USAOs are responsible for entering data into
LIONS, and each
USAO is responsible for certifying, semi-annually, the classifications given in each case. Apparently, there are
problems. The Office of the Inspector General, an oversight agency, stated “that terrorism-related statistics
reported by EOUSA and the USAOs were not accurately reported. This indicates that stronger internal controls
for verifying the accuracy of the LIONS data are needed.”
31
The EOUSA reported processing 1,876 defendants in terrorism-related cases from 2002-2004 (365 defendants in
2002, 786 in 2003, and 725 in 2004).
By contrast, the FBI provided the Center for Terrorism Research with the
names of 525 defendants from September 11, 2001 to August 15, 2004. Using the EOUSA’s own numbers, the
OIG found that the number of terrorism-related cases handled by the department were overstated by 421 people in
2003 and 2004. During that same period the OIG found that the EOUSA overstated the number of terrorism-
related convictions by 402.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
119
EOUSA and the FBI. The EOUSA and FBI, in turn were asked to provide supporting
documentation. In many cases, the OIG and GAO reported that DOJ officials either declined to
provide documentation, citing the classified nature of the information, or they provided
explanations without documentation. Often, the OIC/GAO would disregard those explanations
because the OIG/GAO disagreed with the FBI/EOUSA on what a “viable” link to terrorism
meant.
Either way, it brings up an important problem: an agreed definition of “linked to
terrorism”—or more precisely, an agreement on the goals and objectives of post-9/11
antiterrorism policy and the proper place for diffusion cases. As mentioned above, Attorney
General Ashcroft stated that the objective of the DOJ was to intercept terrorist threats before they
could materialize. While internal memoranda sent to the FBI and EOUSA are not available, the
evidence suggests that diffusion cases were created for the purpose of following the Attorney
General’s mandate. While this might have occurred among the street-level bureaucrats, as a
result of them implementing a vague policy, available evidence suggests that it was a top-down
initiative.
Consider that the timing of the first diffusion cases corresponds to the dramatic increase
in the levels of cooperation between Bureau of Immigration and Customs Enforcement (ICE), a
bureau inside the Department of Homeland Security, and the FBI’s many JTTF offices. Also
consider that diffusion cases have been investigated and referred for prosecution by nearly every
JTTF office in the country. Finally, the first diffusion cases were filed within a month of
Attorney General Ashcroft’s announcement that the objective of antiterrorism policy had become
proactive. The theory behind pursuing diffusion cases comports with Ashcroft’s objectives.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
120
With the available data, one cannot determine whether diffusion cases have been
successful in preventing acts of terrorism. It is a matter of debate whether the pursuit of
diffusion cases is a sound antiterrorism strategy and an efficient use of resources. It is also a
debate that is beyond the scope of this research. One question that is easier to answer, however,
is whether or not these cases are consistent with existing policy. The answer is yes. As
explained in Chapter 6, the Attorney General’s guidelines authorizing terrorism investigations
and the mandate to prosecute cases sooner to prevent acts of terrorism, probably necessitated the
pursuit of diffusion cases. As mentioned in the previous paragraph, evidence suggests that
pursuing diffusion cases was a conscious and deliberate decision by policy makers.
The implementation failure, if any, occurs between the DOJ and the independent agencies
charged with auditing the DOJ’s work: the OIG and GAO. The former is pursuing post-9/11
antiterrorism objectives through new goals established by the executive branch. The latter does
not answer to the executive branch, and appears to be evaluating the DOJ’s work under pre-9/11
measures. Those measures require concrete connections linking those accused to acts of
terrorism and/or an extremist ideology.
This report provides another method of evaluating the policy objectives. By categorizing
cases by type, one can ascertain whether post-9/11 policies have been more effective (i.e.,
resulting in a larger proportion of convictions) than those in place before. As the results show,
even if one ignores diffusion cases altogether, the government has prosecuted slightly more
defendants per year after 9/11. Most of those cases are pretextual, and that is exactly what one
should expect given the mandate to prosecute cases earlier. In addition to more defendants being
prosecuted, the plea bargain rate and the conviction rate have increased in the post-9/11 era.
Again, this effect, while desirable, was probably an unintended consequence of the policy
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
121
changes. It may be the result of different (less politicized) prosecution strategies used by
prosecutors who handled cases with less available evidence.
2. REVIEW OF RESEARCH QUESTIONS AND BACKGROUND
This project began by asking three fundamental research questions. First, what is the
relationship between prosecutorial and defense strategies, and do those strategies affect case
outcomes? Second, how has 9/11 impacted how the federal government responds to terrorism?
Previous research revealed interesting trends indicating that cases involving terrorism defendants
differed from other criminal cases in significant ways. For example, Smith and Damphousse
(1998, 2002) discovered that terrorists were more likely to go to trial than similarly situated non-
terrorists. Existing research had also found that terrorist defendants were sentenced to
significantly longer prison sentences than similarly situated non-terrorist defendants (e.g., Smith
and Damphousse, 1996; 1998; Bradley, Damphousse and Smith, 2008). Such studies suggested
that there may be important differences between terrorists and non-terrorists with regard to
characteristics and processing.
This project focused on policy changes that occurred after 9/11. After that attack,
Congress changed a number of policies to provide tools for combating terrorism. Some policies
expanded the DOJ’s legal authority to intercept, investigate and prosecute domestic terrorists.
For example, the Anti-Terrorism and Effective Death Penalty Act was revisited and the USA
PATRIOT Act of 2001 (Patriot Act) was created to extend and strengthen U.S. antiterrorism
policy. In addition, the executive branch changed Department of Justice policy on how the FBI
and U.S. Attorneys would handle the investigation and prosecution of terror suspects. In a
statement released after September 11, Attorney General Ashcroft explained that the policy of
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
122
the United States government changed from prosecuting terror-related crimes that had already
occurred, to thwarting attacks before they happen (Ashcroft, 2001).
In broad terms, post-9/11 antiterrorism policy is focused on terrorist organizations,
affiliated networks, and state sponsors in an effort to identify potential terrorist threats and
proactively prevent future attacks. These policy changes were met with criticism levied by
government entities, like OIG and the GAO, and members of academia and the media. The
study focused on two critiques specifically—the soft sentence critique, and the data
validitycritique. The former maintains that the DOJ has been overstating its success because
those who have been convicted receive short prison sentences. The latter critique states that the
success of the DOJ is questionable because it routinely includes the convictions of defendants
who are not linked to terrorism groups or acts of terrorism in its terrorism reports.
3. REVIEW OF RESEARCH QUESTION ONE: PROSECUTORIAL AND DEFENSE STRATEGIES
Overall, the relationship between prosecutorial and defense strategies is situational.
While some strategies produce higher conviction rates and others produce lower conviction rates,
the effect of how much lower or higher depends on the combination. Analyses showed that
despite being the most common combination of prosecution strategy and defense strategy,
conventional criminality and traditional defense did not produce the highest conviction rates.
The combination of conventional criminality and either the dissociation defense method, or the
political persecution defense method produced the highest conviction rates. Conventional
criminality produced the highest conviction rates among all prosecution strategies. Political
innuendo was slightly less successful, overall, than conventional criminality.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
123
Even though the conventional criminality prosecution strategy proved to be the most
successful strategy overall, it did so only in a handful of cases where defendants used either the
political innuendo or dissociation defense strategies. The political innuendo prosecution
strategy, when used in cases relying on the political persecution defense produced the highest,
statistically reliable results. The results showed that this outcome was most likely due to the
political persecution defense strategy, which had a positive effect on the probability of
conviction. The lowest conviction rate occurred in the combination of explicit politicality
prosecution strategy and traditional defense strategy.
In fact, among all prosecution strategies, explicit politicality produced the lowest
conviction rates. Similarly, the disassociation defense strategy produced the lowest conviction
rates among all defense strategies. When compared to the overall model average, the two
combined to produce a very low conviction rate, but explicit politicality and a traditional defense
strategy was the combination that resulted in the lowest conviction rate. That trend did not
continue across the different combinations. The findings showed that the disassociation defense
strategy was more successful than the traditional defense strategy when used against the political
innuendo prosecution strategy. Most likely this result occurred because of the nature of political
innuendo cases and the amount and type of evidence that is used to link defendants to terrorism.
The results indicate that using the explicit politicality prosecution strategy presents
prosecutors with the biggest challenge for gaining convictions. The findings produced a
statistically significant negative effect on the likelihood of conviction even when the impact of
evidentiary strength, case complexity, and count severity are controlled. Likewise, defendants
who use the political persecution strategy, regardless of prosecution method, are statistically
more likely to be convicted than when using an alternative defense strategy. But as discussed,
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
124
that may be situational, as it appears easier for defendants to convince a jury that they are the
victims of political persecution when there is a foreign government involved.
4. REVIEW OF RESEARCH QUESTION TWO: EFFECT OF 9/11
Chapter 6 began with a discussion of the three case type categories that were first
described by Robert Chesney (2007). Chesney developed the categories in response to the soft
sentence critique and the data validitycritique that had been raised to question the DOJ’s claims
of success in its antiterrorism policy. Rather than focus on one type of criminal violation, as
Chesney did, this study focused on the policy changes that occurred in the aftermath of 9/11.
Specifically, attention was directed towards two policy changes: first, the Attorney Generals
Guidelines on opening terrorism investigations, and; second, Attorney General Ashcroft’s
mandate for the FBI and EOUSA to begin interrupting terrorist groups by prosecuting defendants
as soon as a criminal case could go forward.
The analyses of the case types suggested that after 9/11 prosecutors relied less heavily on
highly politicized prosecution strategies and filed fewer event-linked cases. As a result, plea
bargain rates and conviction rates increased. This occurred despite the finding that the average
count severity in the post-9/11 era was only slightly lower than it was in the previous era. While
the decision to file less politically charged cases could have been a conscious decision on the part
of prosecutors, it was more likely the result of less evidence—an unintended consequence of
Attorney General Ashcroft’s “prosecute early” policy. The results, showing a dramatic drop in
the number of informants used in the post-9/11 era, provided support for that position.
Turning to diffusion cases, analysis revealed that no diffusion cases were filed before
9/11, yet they made up the majority of cases filed afterwards. Cases within the diffusion
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
125
category may not be the product of poor record keeping as some critics have suggested. Rather,
they appear to be the product of policy changes implemented after 9/11. Attorney General
Ashcroft directed the FBI and the EOUSA to prosecute cases sooner for the purpose of
interrupting terrorists before they could complete attacks. Due to constraints caused by
prosecuting cases sooner, prosecutors began trying to diffuse potential terrorism threats by
targeting crimes that they determined were precursors to terrorism.
While the results of hypothesis testing supported a majority of the hypotheses that were
based on structural contextual theory, some of the hypotheses were not supported. Analysis of
the unsupported hypotheses provided a potential answer to one of the questions that has lingered
in previous terrorism research: why were guilty plea rates in terrorism cases so low in the pre-
9/11 era? The results suggest that the highly politicized prosecution strategies that dominated
federal terrorism trials in the pre-9/11 era may have decreased the likelihood of plea bargaining.
The concept of the ideology effect was introduced to explain how highly politicized cases
increase the contentiousness of terrorism trials because they question the belief systems of
terrorist defendants. The implication is that tightened coupling, a component of structural
contextual theory, does not operate differently in terrorism cases than it does in non-terrorism
cases (leading to lower plea bargain rates in the former while leading to higher plea bargain rates
in the latter), it is simply masked by the ideology effect.
5. SUGGESTED FUTURE RESEARCH
While this project makes a significant contribution to existing terrorism research in two
different areas, there are questions raised in each that deserve closer study. With regard to
prosecutorial and defense strategies, analyses were performed using the broad categories of
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
126
pleadings (motions) that were available. A more in-depth analysis of motion type and their
outcomes could provide better explanations on what effect specific motions have on prosecution
strategies and defense strategies. This level of analysis could also help to improve our
understanding of whether defense attorney type plays a significant role in the type of defense
strategies used.
In addition, the defense strategies included in this project were limited to the three most
common strategies. Future analysis could include each defense strategy to further pinpoint how
defense strategies play into case outcomes. It is also the case that defendants sometimes employ
hybrid defense strategies—they use components of more than one strategy. For this study,
defense strategies were coded for each defendant according to which strategy was used the most.
Creating hybridized categories would probably improve our understanding of the subject.
As with defense strategies, prosecution strategies could be divided further. Recall that
the political innuendo prosecution strategy is used in different degrees and introduced at different
times by prosecutors. Variables measuring when the strategy was employed and how it was
employed would provide a better understand of why this strategy was highly successful when the
explicit politicality strategy was overwhelmingly less successful.
The findings provide valuable insight into what effect 9/11 had on federal antiterrorism
policy. The case type categories provide a useful tool for analyzing antiterrorism policy by
allowing research to be conducted among similar cases. In the aftermath of the major policy
changes that occurred in the months following 9/11, in which policy objectives and goals were
reset, the case type variable exposes the flaws in earlier research which lumped all terrorism
cases into one category and based any findings on pre-9/11 policy goals. But the findings
provided in this study are preliminary.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
127
The most promising area of future study is plea bargaining. It will be important to test
the ideology effect, and the most promising method of doing that is an in-depth analysis of guilty
plea rates. Beyond the predictors that are typically used to explain the decision to enter a guilty
plea, a future analysis should include prosecutorial and defense strategies variables. In addition,
future research should distinguish between the three case types and the various terror types
(right-wing, left-wing, environmental, etc.). It might be possible to construct a surrogate
measure of how devoted a defendant is to his/her ideology by using some of the factors identified
here.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
128
VIII: REFERENCES
Antiterrorism and Effective Death Penalty ACT of 1996, (P.L. 134-132) enacted April 1996.
Ashcroft fact sheet on new FBI investigative guidelines, Politechbot.com, September 21, 2002.
Attorney General’s Guidelines on Domestic Security Investigation, Attorney General Edward
Levi, 1976.
Attorney General's Guidelines on General Crimes, Racketeering Enterprise and Domestic
Security/Terrorism Investigations, Attorney General William French Smith, March 7,
1983
Beam, Louis (1992) The Seditionist, number 12.
Bureau of Justice Statistics (2004) Compendium of Federal Justice Statistics, 2004, found at
http://www.ojp.usdoj.gov/bjs/fed.htm
Burgess, Mark (2003), Terrorism: The Problems of Definition, Center for Defense Information.
Chesney, Robert (2007) “Federal Prosecution of Terrorism-related Cases: Conviction and
Sentencing Data in Light of the “Data Reliability” and “Soft Sentence” Critiques.
Available at http://ssrn.com/abstract_id=1005478
Counterterrorism Threat Assessment and Warning Unit, National Security Division, Federal
Bureau of Investigation, Terrorism in the United States 1999: 30 Years of Terrorism – A
Special Retrospective Edition, Washington, DC: United States Department of Justice,
1999, p i.
Damphousse, Kelly and Chris Shields (2007), The Morning After: Assessing the Effect of Major
Terrorism Events on Prosecution Strategies and Outcomes, Journal of Contemporary
Criminal Justice, Vol. 23, No. 2, 174-194
Damphousse, Kelly and Brent L. Smith (2004), Terrorism and Empirical Testing: Using
Indictment Data to Assess Changes in Terrorist Conduct, Terrorism and Counter-
terrorism; Criminological Perspectives Sociology of Crime, Law and Deviance, Volume
5, 75-90
Department of Justice (2006), Counterterrorism White Paper, (June 22, 2006), available at
http://trac.syr.edu/tracreports/terrorism/169/include/terrorism.whitepaper.pdf
.
Eggen, Dan and Julie Tate (2005), “Few Convictions in Terror Cases; U.S. Often Depends on
Lesser Charges, Washington Post, June 11, 2005
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
129
Fazlollah, Mark, and Peter Nicholas, (2001), “U.S. Overstates Arrests in Terrorism Cases: For
Years, Officials Said, Cases that Should Not Have Been in that Category were Included
to Support Budget Requests”, The Philadelphia Inquirer., Dec. 16, at A1
General Accounting Office. Justice Department, Better Management Oversight and Internal
Controls Needed to Ensure Accuracy of Terrorism-Related Statistics, GAO-03-266,
January 2003
General Accounting Office, Combating Terrorism: Evaluation of Selected Characteristics in
National Strategies Related to Terrorism, U.S. Government Printinging Office.
Washington, D.C., GAO-04-408T, 2004.
General Accounting Office, Federal Law Enforcement, Information on the Use of Investigation
and Arrest Statistics, GAO-04-411, March 2004.
Guelke, Adrian, (1998) The Age of Terrorism and the International Political System, New York:
I. B. Tauris, p. 5.
Heymann, Philip B.(1998) Terrorism and America: A Commonsense Strategy for A Democratic
Society, MIT Press.
Hjern, Benny, (1982) Implementation Research : The Link Gone Missing. Journal of Public
Policy, (2), pp. 301-308.
Laqueur, Walter, (1987) The Age of Terrorism , Boston; Little, Brown and Company, pp. 149-
150.
Lipsky, Michael, (1971) Street level bureaucracy and the Analysis of Urban Reform, Urban
Affairs Quarterly, (6) pp. 391-409.
Mazmanian, D. and Sabatier, P. (1983) Implementation and Public Policy, Glenview: Scott.
National Commission on Terrorist Attacks Upon the United States ( 2004) The 9/11 Commission
Report, W.W. Norton & Company, Inc, New York, NY.
Office of the Coordinator for Counterterrorism, Patterns of Global Terrorism 2002, US
Department of State Publication 11038, Washington , DC : State Department, April
2003, p. 13.
Office of the Inspector General, The Department of Justice's Internal Controls Over Terrorism
Reporting, OIG-07-04, February 2007.
Perl, Rapheal, (2003) “Terrorism, the Future, and American Foreign Policy” Issue Brief for
Congress, Order Code IB95112, Foreign Affairs, Trade and Research Division.
Porch, Douglas and James J,. WIrtz (2002), Surprise and Intelligence Failure, Strategic Insights,
Vol 1, Issue 7 (September).
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
130
Rapoport, David (1977), The Politics of Atrocity, Terrorism: Interdisciplinary Perspectives,
New York: John Jay Press.
Ripley, R.B. and Franklin, G.A. (1982). Bureaucracy and Policy Implementation, Homewood:
Dorsey Press.
Rochefort, David A. and Cobb, Roger W. 1994. The Politics of Problem Definition, Shaping
the Policy Agenda. Lawrence, KS: University of Kansas Press.
Rolince, Michael E. (2003a) testimony as Acting Assistant Director in Charge, FBI
Before the Senate Judiciary Committee on June 24, 2003, The Inspector General's Report
and the September 11th Response.
Rolince, Michael E (2003b) testimony as Acting Assistant Director in Charge, FBI
Before the Senate Judiciary Committee on June 24, 2003, The Inspector General's Report
and the September 11th Response.
Sabatier, P.A. and Mazmanian, D. (1979). The Conditions of Effective Implementation. Policy
Analysis, 5, pp. 481-504.
Saul, Ben (2004), Definition of "Terrorism" in the UN Security Council: 1985–2004, Chinese
Journal of International Law 2005 4(1):141-166.
Shields, Christopher, Kelly Damphousse, and Brent L. Smith (2006), Their Day in Court:
Assessing Plea Bargain Rates Among Terrorists in Federal Trials 1980-2002, Journal of
Contemporary Criminal Justice, Vol. 22, No. 3, 261-276.
Silverlieb, Alan (2008), Poll: Terrorism Fears are Fading, article retrieved from CNN.com on
July1,2008 at http://www.cnn.com/2008/POLITICS/07/02/terrorism.poll/
Smith, Brent L., Kelly R. Damphousse, Song Yang, and Cristie Ginther (2005). “Prosecuting
Politically Motivated Offenders: The Impact of the Terrorist Label on Criminal Case
Outcomes.” International Journal of Contemporary Sociology. 42(2) 209-226.
Smith, Brent and Kelly Damphousse (2002) “American Terrorism Study: Patterns of behavior,
Investigation and Prosecution of American Terrorists, Final Report. NIJ Grant 199-IJ-
CX-0005, submitted 03/27/2002.
Smith, Brent, Kelly Damphousse, Freedom Jackson, and Amy Sellers,(2002), The prosecution
and Punishment of International Terrorists in Federal Courts: 1980-1998, Criminology &
Public Policy, Vol. 1 (3): 311-338.
Smith, Brent & Kelly Damphousse (1998) Terrorism, Politics, and Punishment: A Test of
Structural-Contextual Theory and the “liberation Hypothesis”, Criminology, Vol. 36, No.
1: 67-92.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
131
Stern, Jessica (1999) The Ultimate Terrorists, Harvard University Press, p.11.
Transactional Record Access Clearinghouse (2003) “Criminal Terrorism Enforcement Since the
9/11/01 Attacks” A TRAC Special Report, December 8, found at
http://trac.syr.edu/tracreports/terrorism/ report031208.html
Turk, Austin (1982) Political Criminality. Beverley Hills, CA:Sage.
Uniting and Strengthening America by Providing Appropriate Tools Required to Intercept and
Obstruct Terrorism Act, (P.L. 107-56) enacted October 2001.
United States Department of Defense, Office of Joint Chiefs of Staff, Joint Publication 1-02:
Department of Defense Dictionary of Military and Associated Terms , Washington , DC :
United States Department of Defense, 12 April 2001 – As amended through 5 June 2003,
p. 531.
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
132
Appendix 1
Count Severity Codes
(Federal A.O. code is in parentheses):
Treason, sedition (9754) = 29
Murder, 1
st
(0100) = 28
Kidnapping, hostage (7611) = 27
Racketeering (7400) = 26
Explosives (994) = 25
Firearms (7380) = 24
Robbery, bank (1100) = 23
Murder, 1
st
, conspiracy ( 0101) = 22
Embezzlement, bankruptcy (4990) = 21
Counterfeiting (5800) = 20
Robbery, conspiracy (1400) = 19
Manslaughter (0300) = 18
Firearms, machine guns, conspiracy (7800) = 17
Drugs, cocaine (6701) = 16
Drugs, distribution marijuana (6501) = 15
Auto theft (5100) = 14
Embezzlement, other (4990) = 13
Theft, bank (3100) = 12
National defense (9790) = 11
Racketeering, arson, conspiracy (7410) = 10
Embezzlement , postal/wire (4700) = 9
Theft, transportation, conspiracy (3600) = 8
Escape (7312) = 7
Aiding escapee (7320) = 6
Theft, U.S. property, conspiracy (3400) = 5
Embezzlement, false claims (4991) = 4
Firearms, possession (7820) = 3
Contempt (9921) = 2
Miscellaneous (9999) = 1
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
133
Appendix 2
Average Count Severity by Prosecution Strategy
prosmeth recode Mean N Std. Deviation
conventional criminality
17.81 1917 5.109
political innuendo
18.20 947 8.784
explicit politicality
23.27 6438 5.175
N= 9302, F( 2 , 9229)= 866.1, p < .001
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
134
Appendix 3
Percentage of Conspiracy Counts by Prosecution Strategy
prosmeth recode Mean N Std. Deviation
conventional criminality
.03 2015 .171
political innuendo
.06 1012 .238
explicit politicality
.19 6598 .391
Total
.14 9625 .349
N =9625, F ( 2, 9622) = 196.5, p < .001
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
135
Appendix 4
Crosstab Count Outcome by Defense Strategy
Defense Strategy
ctresult_recode Total
convict plead
dismiss after
guilty plea dismiss acquit convict
political
persecution
Count
% within defmeth
% within ctresult
372
41.0%
48.1%
70
7.7%
13.2%
181
20.0%
7.0%
263
29.0%
54.6%
21
2.3%
15.4%
907
100.0%
20.1%
disassociation
Count
% within defmeth
% within ctresult
208
20.5%
26.9%
145
14.3%
27.3%
458
45.1%
17.7%
103
10.1%
21.4%
101
10.0%
74.3%
1015
100.0%
22.5%
traditional
Count
% within defmeth
% within ctresult
193
7.5%
25.0%
316
12.2%
59.5%
1942
75.2%
75.2%
116
4.5%
24.1%
14
.5%
10.3%
2581
100.0%
57.3%
Total
Count
% within defmeth
% within ctresult
773
17.2%
100.0%
531
11.8%
100.0%
2581
57.3%
100.0%
482
10.7%
100.0%
136
3.0%
100.0%
4503
100.0%
100.0%
Pearson x
2
= 1450.5, df 8, P < .001
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
136
Appendix 5
Average number of counts dropped per defendant in Superseding Indictments
Defense Method Mean N Std. Deviation
political persecution
.18 93 .488
disassociation
1.91 164 6.253
traditional
.77 253 5.323
Total
1.03 510 5.197
N = 6813, F (2, 4162)= 29.432, p < .001
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
137
Appendix 6
Average Count Severity by Defense Strategy
defmeth_recode Mean N Std. Deviation
political persecution
19.92 1,137 7.523
disassociation
19.56 1,001 8.394
traditional
19.76 3,013 5.973
N= 5161, F( 2, 5158)= .757, p= .469
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
138
Appendix 7
Counts Charged in Diffusion Cases by USC Chapter
Frequency Percent
Valid crimes/general provisions
3 .2
claims & services in matters affecting gov't
1 .1
counterfeiting & forgery
1 .1
embezzlement & theft
1 .1
explosive materials
2 .1
firearms
16 .8
fraud/ false statements
29 1.5
mail fraud
185 9.6
passports & visas
39 2.0
racketeering
144 7.5
RICO
54 2.8
stolen property
17 .9
release & detention pending judicial
proceedings
1 .1
immigration & nationality
8 .4
drug abuse prevention & control
19 1.0
machine guns, destructive devices, other
firearms
4 .2
crimes, other offenses & forfeitures
7 .4
monetary transactions
1,312 68.0
social security
13 .7
aviation programs
32 1.7
nationality & citizenship
7 .4
bribery/graft
22 1.1
food stamp fraud
2 .1
interfere fair housing
3 .2
transporting hazardous material
3 .2
prohibited transactions
2 .1
Total
1,927 99.9
Missing System
2 .1
Total
1,929 100.0
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
139
Appendix 8
Average Number of Pro se Motion Filed RW Cases
ERA Mean N
Pre-Leaderless Resistance
4.91 35
Leaderless Resistance
8.95 60
t(93)= -1.75, p < .10
Proportion Right-wing Defendants who Filed Pro se Motions
ERA Mean N
Pre-Leaderless Resistance
.14 95
Leaderless Resistance
.40 134
t(227)= -4.681, p < .001
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
140
Appendix 9
Crosstab case-type X Case outcome 5
th
category added
Case Outcomes Total
Case Type
trial
conviction
plea
guilty acquittal dismiss
charges
pending
/Fugitive
case
type
event
linked
Count
5 28 2 5 10 50
Expected Count
3.9 35.3 .9 3.0 6.9 50.0
% within case
type
10.0% 56.0% 4.0% 10.0% 20.0% 100.0%
% within
outcome1
38.5% 23.7% 66.7% 50.0% 43.5% 29.9%
% of Total
3.0% 16.8% 1.2% 3.0% 6.0% 29.9%
pretextual Count
3 22 0 2 11 38
Expected Count
3.0 26.9 .7 2.3 5.2 38.0
% within case
type
7.9% 57.9% .0% 5.3% 28.9% 100.0%
% within
outcome1
23.1% 18.6% .0% 20.0% 47.8% 22.8%
% of Total
1.8% 13.2% .0% 1.2% 6.6% 22.8%
diffusion Count
5 68 1 3 2 79
Expected Count
6.1 55.8 1.4 4.7 10.9 79.0
% within case
type
6.3% 86.1% 1.3% 3.8% 2.5% 100.0%
% within
outcome1
38.5% 57.6% 33.3% 30.0% 8.7% 47.3%
% of Total
3.0% 40.7% .6% 1.8% 1.2% 47.3%
Total Count
13 118 3 10 23 167
Expected Count
13.0 118.0 3.0 10.0 23.0 167.0
% within case
type
7.8% 70.7% 1.8% 6.0% 13.8% 100.0%
% within
outcome1
100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0% 100.0%
% of Total
7.8% 70.7% 1.8% 6.0% 13.8% 100.0%
Terrorism Research Center in Fulbright College
141